Skip to main content

Evaluating a new verbal working memory-balance program: a double-blind, randomized controlled trial study on Iranian children with dyslexia

Abstract

Background

It is important to improve verbal Working Memory (WM) in reading disability, as it is a key factor in learning. There are commercial verbal WM training programs, which have some short-term effects only on the verbal WM capacity, not reading. However, because of some weaknesses in current verbal WM training programs, researchers suggested designing and developing newly structured programs that particularly target educational functions such as reading skills. In the current double-blind randomized clinical trial study, we designed a new Verbal Working Memory-Balance (VWM-B) program which was carried out using a portable robotic device. The short-term effects of the VWM-B program, on verbal WM capacity, reading skills, and postural control were investigated in Iranian children with developmental dyslexia.

Results

The effectiveness of the VWM-B program was compared with the VWM-program as a traditional verbal WM training. In comparison with VWM-program, the participants who received training by the VWM-B program showed superior performance on verbal WM capacity, reading skills, and postural control after a short-term intervention.

Conclusions

We proposed that the automatized postural control resulting from VWM-B training had a positive impact on improving verbal WM capacity and reading ability. Based on the critical role of the cerebellum in automatizing skills, our findings support the cerebellar deficit theory in dyslexia.

Trial registration: This trial was (retrospectively) registered on 8 February 2018 with the Iranian Registry of Clinical Trials (IRCT20171219037953N1).

Peer Review reports

Background

Developmental Dyslexia (DD) is characterized as a difficulty in learning to read accurately and fluently [1], despite adequate intelligence, conventional classroom experience, and sufficient socio-economic opportunities [2]. 5–17.5% of children in different countries suffer from DD [3, 4]. As shown below, several theories and approaches have described DD.

Many studies have supported that reading difficulties in children with DD are due to phonological deficits, explained by the phonological deficit theory [1, 5, 6]. This theory has suggested that children with DD have a specific impairment in the representation, storage, and retrieval of speech sounds (phonological awareness problems) [7]. The phonological awareness problems lead to difficulties in grapheme–phoneme decoding of the lexical items. Insufficient grapheme–phoneme decoding causes the slowing process and inadequate recognition of letters [1, 5], leading to problems in segmentation and blending. Impaired phonological representations also limit the formation of long-term phonological representations in restoring phonological (verbal) Working Memory (WM) traces [8]. Hence, children with DD usually have deficits in verbal WM in addition to problems in phonological awareness, grapheme–phoneme decoding and segmentation [9, 10]. The verbal WM engages the phonological loop component of WM and involves the temporary maintenance and manipulation of auditory-verbal information via vocal/subvocal rehearsal [11]. Extensive evidence has confirmed the existence of the verbal WM deficit in children with DD as a fundamental problem [9, 12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22]. The verbal WM deficit in these children may extend into adulthood and thereafter affect performance in all components of WM [23]. Therefore, sufficient improvement of the verbal WM capacity in children with DD is necessary [24,25,26].

The cerebellar deficit theory, supported by several studies, has concluded that insufficient integration of information due to mild neurobiological impairment in the cerebellum is responsible for deficits in DD [16, 27,28,29]. This theory has postulated that retarded or dysfunctional articulation due to a weak capacity to automatize would lead to deficient phonological representations and affect the learning of grapheme–phoneme decoding [7, 30, 31]. As mentioned above, the impaired phonological representations limit the formation of long-term phonological representations in restoring verbal WM traces [8]. Furthermore, research has confirmed the imperative role of the cerebellum in verbal WM [32]. Unlike other approaches, the cerebellar deficits theory emphasizes postural control and balance disorders in children with DD besides supporting phonological and verbal WM deficits [1, 9, 16, 27, 33]. Cerebellar insufficiency leads to difficulties in developing automatized skills [16]. Due to incomplete automaticity, balance-related problems become apparent while performing dual-tasks or more complex tasks [34]. In dual-tasks, children with DD are unable to consciously compensate for both the cognitive or motor aspects of dual-tasks [34]. Dual-task paradigm studies have shown both postural control [27, 33, 35] and cognitive performance insufficiency [27, 33] in children with DD. The postural control and cognitive demands, therefore, interact with each other in a cognitive-motor dual-task [27, 33, 35]. There is evidence that dual-task interference decreases and may even disappear while performing a dual-task condition [36]. In other words, dual-task training can improve dual-task performance [37] and this forms the motivation for the current study. Also, a cognitive-motor dual-task training program is more efficient than a single-task program to improve cognitive or motor performances [38,39,40,41] (e.g., balance performance [40, 42]). Hence, we have supposed that a training program with a dual-task condition could be more effective than a single-task program to develop the abilities in children with DD.

Numerous computerized programs including Cogmed (www.cogmed.com), Jungle Memory (www.junglememory.com), and Cognifit (www.cognifit.com) are currently used to improve the verbal WM capacity [43]. These programs are typically commercial, and several studies have taken place to examine their effectiveness [25, 43,44,45,46,47]. Some studies demonstrated the positive effects of these programs on reading ability [25, 44]. However, many researchers have confirmed the short-term effects of these programs only on the verbal WM capacity [43, 45,46,47,48], and in comparison to the other programs, Cogmed has larger effects on the verbal WM capacity [43]. Moreover, the current programs suffer from some weaknesses. They were not designed to teach the verbal WM explicit strategies, such as vocal/subvocal rehearsal techniques [49], and designed as a single-task training program that leads to specific-to-practice learning effects [48]. Because of these weaknesses, researchers suggested designing and developing newly structured WM programs that particularly target educational functions such as reading skills [43, 47, 50]. It seems that designing a new effective verbal WM training program for children with DD should be adapted to cover difficulties in balance and automaticity.

The current study hypothesizes that a new dual-task program that involves explicit strategies of vocal/subvocal rehearsal and targets the reading skills and balance-related performance simultaneously, would be more effective than the current programs to improve the verbal WM capacity, reading skills, or postural control. In essence, the research design compared the progress of dyslexic children who either experienced a WM battery (control group) or the same WM battery under dual-task balance conditions (intervention group) which are hypothesized to improve performance. In the present study, a dual-task, Verbal Working Memory-Balance (VWM-B) program, was designed and its short-term effects on verbal WM capacity, reading skills, and postural control were investigated in Iranian children with DD.

Methods

Subjects and design

This quasi double-blind randomized clinical trial study was performed with a between-subjects factor ‘group’ (control group vs. intervention group) and within-subjects factor ‘time’ (measurement at pre-intervention and post-intervention) and has adhered to CONSORT guidelines (Additional file 1: Appendix S1). Data collection started in March 2018 and ended in November 2018. First, an invitation letter was sent from the ‘education office, District 20, Tehran, Iran’ to the principals of the public elementary schools in this region for referring students with reading deficiency (Fig. 1). After the invitation, children with difficulties in learning to read, reported by their teachers or those with a previous diagnosis of DD, were participated in preliminary screening. Then, the word reading efficiency (WR) and non-word reading efficiency (NWR) subtests of the validated and reliable Persian battery of normative reading tests—NEMA [51] were used to confirm the existence of DD. Children who obtained a score of 25% or less for WR and NWR subtests in the preliminary screening were included in the study [51]. Non-words are particularly important in diagnosing dyslexia for those who follow the phonological deficit hypothesis [52]. It should be noted that Iranian children begin to learn to read at the age of 4–5 years when they participate in preschool classes [53]. Preschool is an informal education course in Iran (under the supervision of the education ministry), and mandatory (at least for 1 year) before receiving a formal education course [53]. Iranian children, older than 6 years, attend the first grade of elementary school and formally learn to read [53]. With this explanation, we have requested teachers to report their students who had learning problems to read after at least 6 months of education in the first-grade. Therefore, children in the first grade of the school should have received at least 18 months of education services. However, all first-grade participants in the present study had received more than 21 months of education services.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A flow chart illustrating the procedure of recruiting the participants

Inclusion criteria were normal IQ, normal attention, normal vision/hearing conditions, right-handed, native-Persian language, and average socio-economic status as reported by the families. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) was used to test the IQ and subjects with a WISC-IV total score ˂ 85 excluded from the study [54]. Also, the Persian version of the parent checklist of the Child Symptoms Inventory (CSI-4) was utilized to test attention (items 1 to 18 out of 97) [55, 56]. Subjects with total scores of 1–18 items ≥ 7 were excluded from the study [55,56,57]. Furthermore, none of the children had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and were taking no drugs affecting the central nervous system.

According to Fig. 1, which illustrates the procedure of recruiting participants in accordance with CONSORT guidelines [58], 36 children with DD were recruited to the study following formal diagnostic and behavioral pre-intervention assessments. However, with an approximate drop-out rate of 20%, data collected from 29 subjects entered the statistical analysis. The current study sample size is consistent with similar previous studies [44, 59,60,61] and is supported by Julious et al. who suggested at least 12 subjects per group in trial studies [62]. The Block randomization method in a 1:1 ratio was performed, by a computer, to allocate participants into two groups [63]. Randomization was performed in blocks of six and a block size of four to ensure a balance in sample size across groups. Also, both groups were matched by age (years), height (cm), weight (kg), full-IQ score (tested by WISC-IV [54]), and attention (tested by CSI-4 [57]), as possible confounders.

For double-blinding in the current study, children and their parents were unaware of the group to which the children had been allocated. Also, an evaluator who was not a member of the research group blinded to the subjects’ groups performed the pre-intervention and post-intervention behavioral assessments. Recording the Center of Pressure (CoP) data using a force plate (more details are given in Section “Assessments” in the “Methods”) was performed on the same day with the behavioral measures. The analyzer of CoP data was also blinded to the allocated intervention. Despite blinding children/parents and evaluator/analyzer to the allocated intervention and pre-intervention/post-intervention assessments, the participants would obviously recognize whether or not they had undertaken the training in the balance condition. Hence, the study design may be considered quasi double-blind.

Children were assessed individually at initial screening, pre-intervention, and post-intervention, separated by an average of 44 days. Outcomes of the diagnostic reading subtests, obtained from the initial screening, were used as the pre-intervention score for children who were recruited for the study. All participants in both groups completed 5 weeks, 3 days per week, one session per day, and 45–60 min per session intervention. Failing to complete a minimum of 75% of the training sessions, i.e., four out of 15 sessions, has been determined to exclude the participant’s data from the statistical analysis (all subjects, however, participated in all 15 sessions).

Training programs

Based on Baddeley’s theory, verbal WM includes encoding, maintenance and manipulation of verbal information, and retrieval sub-processes [11]. Fundamental steps of programs, used in the present study, were developed with respect to this theory. In the current research, participants in control and intervention groups received training using VWM-program and VWM-B program, respectively. The VWM-program included verbal WM all sub-processes and considered as a form of the current training programs [11, 43]. A portable robotic device was also adopted and developed to perform the newly designed VWM-B program (Fig. 2a and b). The robot consisted of a platform that could be programmed to perform any desired tilting motion in the range of 0°–20° in both anteroposterior and mediolateral, or in a combination of both (Fig. 2c and d). The platform was also equipped with a force plate, with a sampling frequency of 100 Hz and an accuracy of ± 0.4 mm, to measure the CoP [64]. The setup had also a computerized interface using a 19-inch touch screen monitor, and a speaker. The computerized interface ran software that was specially designed for the proposed training program.

Fig. 2
figure 2

A robotic device was adopted to implement the new VWM-B training program. a An overview of the robot, b The whole setup while the subject performs a training trial, c The motorized tilting platform and the force plate, d An example of the tilting function

VWM-program for participants in the control group

While training with the VWM-program, the subject sat on a chair in a relaxed mood with arms resting on the table. A 19-inch touch screen monitor ran the software, which was specially designed for the proposed training program, and a speaker was used to recite the words. As mentioned above, each training trial of the VWM-program includes all three sub-processes (encoding, maintenance, and retrieval steps) of the verbal WM. Each trial began 3 s after touching the start button on the monitor (see the start/stop button in Fig. 3a). For the encoding step, the target, which could be a word, a series of words, or a statement, written inside a box (target box), was shown on the monitor for 10 s (Fig. 3a). At the same time, the target was recited by playing a pre-recorded voice on the computer. For the maintenance step, the target was decomposed to its components (sentence to its words or word to its letters) and shown on the monitor inside separate boxes (component boxes) for 10 s (Fig. 3b). Finally, for the retrieval step, twice as many boxes, which included the practiced components and new ones appeared on the monitor (Fig. 3c). The participant had 10 s to select and touch the boxes, which had appeared and been recited as a component of the target.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Training steps of the Verbal Working Memory (VWM) and VWM-Balance (VWM-B) programs. a Encoding step that is similar in both programs. b In the maintenance step, the target box decomposes to component boxes. The center of pressure (CoP) marker and start position button are displayed on the monitor only in the VWM-B program. In the VWM-program, a subject observes the monitor and attempts to maintain targets in the memory. However, in the VWM-B program, the maintenance step is performed in two forms: passive and active balance. In the passive state, the component box automatically hit by the CoP marker; however, in the active state, the subject actively moves the CoP marker to hit the component boxes. c For the retrieval step in the VWM-program, the target is shown and the user should accept or reject recalling the target. d In the VWM-B program’s retrieval step, the subject has to move his CoP to select the target

VWM-B program for participants in the intervention group

As stated above, the newly designed VWM-B program has been performed using the robotic device (Fig. 2). Like the VWM-program, each training trial in the VWM-B program includes all three steps of verbal WM. The difference is that balance movements have been combined with the maintenance and retrieval steps of verbal WM. Before the training session, the amplitude of the CoP movement was calibrated to each participant's limit of stability for safety [65]. Participants’ standing condition was controlled for uniformity among subjects. The feet position on the platform was the same for all participants, with an approximate distance of 10 cm between the feet. Also, the monitor was located at eye level, with a distance of approximately 50 cm (Fig. 2b).

For the encoding step, similar to the VWM-program, a trial began 3 s after touching the start button on the monitor, and the main target box appeared on the monitor for 10 s (Fig. 3a). Then, the component boxes appeared on the screen. In addition to the component boxes, a red circle (CoP marker) also appeared on the screen (Fig. 3b). This circle represented the position of the subject's CoP and was used to introduce balance tasks to the program.

As a new method, training the maintenance and manipulation of information was performed in two forms: passive and active balance. In the passive state, the motorized moving platform underneath the subject’s feet was tilted and the CoP marker was correspondingly moved toward the component boxes implying a passive exercise (see Figs. 2b, d, and 3b). After the component box was hit by the CoP marker, the participant had 10 s to recite the word inside the box. Then, the platform and the CoP marker returned to the start position (Fig. 3b). This procedure was repeated for all component boxes in the correct order. In the active state, the platform had no tilting motion, and the subject had to actively move his CoP towards the component boxes using ankle/hip strategies. After hitting each component box, the participant attempted to read the word aloud without time limitation. Following reading the word, he returned to the start position and repeated the procedure for all component boxes. In sum, the maintenance step of the VWM-program was 10 s. However, the maintenance step in the VWM-B program included two phases: (1) the passive state that limited for 10 s, and (2) the active state that the subject had free time to read the word. Retrieving information in the VWM-B program was similar to the VWM-program except that the subject had 10 s to select the response option using his CoP movements (Fig. 3d).

In sum, the encoding step is similar in both programs. Also, the target box decomposes to component boxes in the maintenance step of both two programs. In the VWM-program, a subject observes the monitor and attempts to maintain targets in the memory. However, in the VWM-B program, the maintenance step is performed in two forms: passive and active balance. In the passive state, the component box is automatically hit by the CoP marker; however, in the active state, the subject actively moves the CoP marker to hit the component boxes. For the retrieval step in the VWM-program, the target is shown and the user should accept or reject recalling the target. However, in the VWM-B program's retrieval step, the subject has to move his CoP to select the target.

To make training trials progressively more difficult in both programs and selecting suitable words to practice, we considered factors that impacted on the verbal WM, including phonological similarity, word length, articulatory suppression, and irrelevant sound effects [11]. Training trials for sessions were determined based on the subject’s capacity in verbal WM and reading. In the maintenance step of the VWM-B program, the main target box was decomposed to 2–9 component boxes (Fig. 3b). In the passive state, the duration of the marker/platform displacement was adjustable between 3 and 10 s. Also, to further the balance challenge, the component boxes were placed at different distances from the start position button (Fig. 3b).

Assessments

Five subtests of the NEMA [51] were used to confirm the existence of DD and provide the pre-intervention and post-intervention assessments. The selected NEMA subtests included the WR, NWR, phoneme deletion, text comprehension, and word chain. Also, the oldest and widely used measure of Forward Digit Span (FDS) was employed to test the verbal WM capacity [66, 67].

The Stroop color-word test was used to measure changes in selective attention [68,69,70]. This validated test includes three components. In the color-naming component, the subject is asked to name the color of 176 bars with colors of red, blue, green, and yellow. In the word-naming component, the subject is asked to read a series of color words including 176 words with colors of red, blue, green, and yellow. Here, the subject reads the word by ignoring its color. In the color-word component, the subject is asked to name the color of words presented in the word-naming component by ignoring their printed form [68, 71]. In the current research, the time for each component was recorded. Then, the color-word interference was calculated as the time of the third component minus the time of the second component [71].

To assess the postural stability, the CoP data were collected with a portable customized force plate [64]. The force plate was linked via a cable connection to the computer. Data were collected at a sampling rate of 100 Hz [64]. Recording of the CoP data was performed in a quiet stance with two conditions eyes open and closed. For each condition, two recordings, with a duration of 70 s, was recorded and the mean value of parameters were used [72]. In eyes open, the subject’s gaze was fixed at a cross mark that was placed on a wall four meters ahead [73]. During the test, participants stood without shoes, with arms folded across the chest. Feet position on the force plate were also marked for inter-trial repeatability. They were instructed to remain still and relaxed in the given stance.

The CoP parameters are suitable to measure postural control in children with DD [29, 74]. We analyzed the validated parameters of the CoP including the surface area (ellipse with 95% of CoP excursions), the length (the path length of the CoP), the mean velocity, and the standard deviation in anteroposterior and mediolateral directions [72, 73, 75]. These are efficient measures of the CoP spatial variability and good indices of the amount of neuromuscular activity required to regulate postural control [29, 76]. After removing the first and the last 5 s of data, the signals were low-pass filtered using a 4th order Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 10 Hz [65, 73, 77]. Data was analyzed using Matlab R2016b (MathWorks, MA, USA).

Analysis

The normality was tested by Shapiro–Wilk [78]. Depending on the distribution of variables, independent t-test for parametric variables, Mann–Whitney U-test for non-parametric variables, and chi-square test for categorical variables were used to compare groups at baseline (α = 0.05). The mean (SD) for quantitative variables and the absolute frequency (%) for qualitative variables were reported. In the current study, both two groups had received the intervention. Hence, the mixed between-within ANOVA analyses were used to verify the treatment effects over time [79]. Significant interactions (p < 0.05) and effect size, with partial eta squared (ηp2), were reported [79]. The ηp2 is in the family of correlational effect size [80] and is most useful for comparing effect sizes in mixed designs [81]. There are no agreed standards for how to interpret an effect size. Nevertheless, 0.2 is considered a small improvement, 0.5 medium, and 0.8 and above large [80]. Using mixed ANOVA, the effects of training programs on the verbal WM (tested by FDS), reading skills (tested by WR, NWR, phoneme deletion, text comprehension, and word chain subtests of the NEMA), attention (tested by Stroop test), and CoP parameters including the surface area, the length, the mean velocity, and the standard deviation in anteroposterior and mediolateral directions were analyzed. Pearson’s correlations between the entire sample gain scores (i.e., the difference between scores in the measurements at pre-intervention and post-intervention) were also reported to explore relationships between the clinical and CoP measures. The SPSS 21 (SPSS Statistics, version 11, IBM, and Armonk, New York, USA) was used to analyze the data.

Results

The control group contained 14 children with DD, with a mean (SD) age of 8 (1.22) years. The Intervention group included 15 children with DD, with a mean (SD) age of 8 (0.96) years. The hypotheses of normality for height, weight, and full-IQ scores were accepted (p > 0.05), and the independent t-test showed no significant between-group difference at baseline scores. However, the hypotheses of normality for age and CSI-4 scores were rejected (p < 0.05), and the Mann–Whitney U-test showed no significant between-group difference at baseline scores. As a result, two groups were homogenized (p > 0.05) for age, height, weight, full-IQ, and attention, as possible confounders (Table 1). Further information about the demographic characteristics of participants is shown in Table 1. Also, no significant difference (p > 0.05) was found for baseline scores of clinical and CoP measures (see t-test outcomes in Table 2). Table 2 also presents the alteration in the mean (SD) of clinical and CoP measures outcomes after the intervention.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics
Table 2 Mean (standard deviation) of the clinical and center of pressure measures

The mixed ANOVA was used to assess the impact of training programs on participants’ scores on the FDS, NEMA subtests, and CoP measures (for more information see Table 3). The time main effect was significant for outcome measures of the FDS, reading subtests, and the mean velocity parameter of the CoP in the eyes-closed condition. These results indicate the alteration in scores after the intervention, regardless of the participants’ group. The group main effect was significant for outcome measures of the FDS, WR, and text comprehension subtests of the NEMA. These results demonstrate that scores of these measures changed in groups, regardless of the time effect. The time × group interaction was also significant for all measures of the FDS, reading subtests, and CoP parameters, except the length, mean velocity, and the standard deviation of mediolateral direction in the eyes-closed condition. When the time × group interaction is significant, it means that there are differences between the two groups over time.

Table 3 Outcomes of mixed between-within ANOVA analyses

Pearson's correlation coefficients of the entire sample are reported in Table 4. Some CoP measures in eyes-open and closed conditions were correlated with FDS. The CoP measures, especially in the eyes-open condition, were correlated with reading subtests. The FDS was correlated with reading subtests. The Stroop test was also correlated only with the WR subtest of NEMA.

Table 4 Pearson correlation between measures r (p-value)

We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for highlighting a slight school-grade difference, which was not significant between the control and the intervention group (see Table 1). Keeping in mind clarification on the teaching system (see more information in Section “Subjects and design” in the “Methods”) and in response to this suggestion, we ran further ANOVA analyses on children in grades 2 and 3, which produced the same pattern of significant results (Tables 5 and 6).

Table 5 Outcomes of mixed between-within ANOVA analyses for the sum of the second and third grade students, when the first-graders were excluded
Table 6 Mean (standard deviation) of the clinical and center of pressure measures for the sum of the second and third grade students, when the first-graders were excluded

Discussion

In the current research, we aimed to examine the effectiveness of the VWM-B program on verbal WM capacity, reading ability, and postural control in children with DD. In comparison with VWM-program, the VWM-B program showed superior performance on verbal WM capacity, reading skills, and postural control after a short-term intervention.

Based on our best knowledge, VWM-B is the only training program, which contains a mix of cognitive and balance-related performance simultaneously, which has been used in DD. Previous studies have also reported some positive effects for a combination of cognitive and physical training in other populations [82,83,84]. Regarding the sequential nature of the process in a dual-task condition, e.g., the VWM-B program, the nervous system first prioritizes a task and then assigns further cognitive/attentional resources for the prioritized task. Therefore, the performance decreases on the non-priority task [85]. Furthermore, sufficient manipulating and maintaining information in the verbal WM is critical for increasing verbal WM capacity [86]. Keeping these points in mind, the featured maintenance step of the VWM-B program probably had an important role in efficiently improving the measured functions in children with DD (see Section “VWM-B program for participants in the intervention group” in the “Methods”). We designed the maintenance step of the VWM-B program within two passive and active balance states of the subject. In the passive state, we designed the cognitive task as a prioritized task. In the active state, however, the balance was considered a prioritized task. Hence, we expected the balance-related movements would be automatized [75], and as a result, further resources assign to the cognitive task [85]. The cerebellar deficit hypothesis in dyslexia characterizes the behavioral symptoms of dyslexia as difficulties in skills automatization [16]. Based on this theory, the cerebellum is a key structure in the automatization deficits [87]. Therefore, it appears that the positive effects of the VWM-B program on the measured functions stemmed from its effects on cerebellum activation.

The present study showed improvement in the intervention group participants’ postural control after the intervention, which was perceived in both eyes open and closed conditions of CoP. It indicates that the balance-related movements were automatized after the intervention. The older evidence revealed that there is no significant difference between the upright standing postural control (eyes open) of the dyslexic and non-dyslexic children [88]. However, children with DD have weaker postural control when they use visual information to perform an activity (actions often are complex or dual-task) [34, 88]. The reason is supposedly insufficient coupling of the visual inputs and postural sway while performing an activity [88]. The improved postural control in the eyes-open condition demonstrates that coupling visual information and body sway were probably improved, and the intervention group participants could assign sensory information to produce purposeful actions [88]. In other words, these participants showed higher performance in using non-visual information to maintain postural control and benefit from the visual information to perform purposeful (cognitive) actions. The improved postural control in the eyes-closed condition implies that the intervention group participants probably benefited from vestibular and/or proprioceptive information and were less dependent on visual inputs to maintain postural control [89]. Since there were no significant changes in the CoP measures of the control group participants, it is concluded that the motor strategies relating to balance control were automatized [75, 88, 90, 91], and further neural resources were allocated to the cognitive task following the intervention by the VWM-B program [85, 90, 91].

In the present study, participants who had received training by the VWM-B program showed higher performance in verbal WM and reading skills. Also, the current research demonstrated that improved postural control was correlated with verbal WM and reading ability. Furthermore, verbal WM was correlated with reading ability. Although the current research has emphasized the cerebellar deficits hypothesis to interpret the results, the causal link between the balance deficits and reading problems is still under controversy [74, 92,93,94]. On the other hand, literature has confirmed the critical role of the cerebellum in verbal WM deficits [32]. Hence, it appears that the automatized postural control initially caused an improvement in verbal WM capacity; and thereupon, the increased verbal WM capacity has led to an improvement in reading ability [95]. It seems that the improved verbal WM capacity facilitated the word and non-word reading ability via improved phonological awareness (tested by the WR and phoneme deletion subtests of NEMA) [96, 97]. Also, improvement in the grapheme–phoneme decoding (tested by the NWR and word chain subtests of NEMA) and phonemic awareness (tested by the WR and phoneme deletion subtests of NEMA) may be related to an improvement in reading comprehension (tested by the text comprehension subtest of NEMA) [97, 98]. In the case of attention, the close link between WM capacity and attention has been confirmed by previous studies [99]. It has been reported that the WM modulates attention [69, 99, 100], and on the other hand, that attention promotes the encoding, maintaining, and manipulating of information in the WM [101, 102]. Despite these pieces of evidence, the Stroop test outcomes in the present study were uncorrelated with verbal WM capacity. Decreasing in the Stroop interference was correlated only with the WR subtest of NEMA. The significantly decreased Stroop interference in the intervention group participants could be justified by the structure of Stroop test. The word-naming step of the Stroop needs the subject to read the colored words [68]. It shows the subjects’ reading rate and reflects their speech-motor problems [68, 71]. Children with DD usually spend further time to complete this component [68, 70]. Keeping this point in mind, the decreased Stroop interference in the current study probably resulted from the improvement in word reading ability (tested by the WR subtest of NEMA). This claim is supported by previous studies when they declared that decreased Stroop interference implies improvements in reading ability as well as selective attention [68,69,70].

The cerebellar deficit hypothesis has also emphasized the impairments in the procedural learning system, with specific deficits in the language/cerebellar procedural circuits [87]. Almost all human activity needs trial-and-error (supervised) learning, which is a sub-type of procedural learning [87]. The cerebellum is a central structure in human brain circuits, and it is a crucial point that only the cerebellum has a hub circuitry to support supervised learning [87]. It implies that if this type of learning is required (e.g., in reading), it is necessary to involve the cerebellum as part of the circuit, along with the other parts of the brain involved in reading [87] (e.g., cortical regions of perisylvian [6] and prefrontal [23] involved in phonological processing and verbal WM, respectively). The corticocerebellar circuits involve loops from the cortex to the cerebellum to thalamic nuclei and back to the cortex [103]. Insufficient skill automatization due to impaired cerebellar function leads to problems in reading, though via different cerebellar circuits [104]. Considering the findings of the current study for CoP, it seems that the VWM-B program caused changes in the activation of cerebellum circuitry. Some regions of the cerebellum may be activated in this dual-task performance [36] and could integrate motor and cognitive networks and adjust these networks to be more efficient for performing the dual-task properly [36].

Although it needs future neuroimaging studies to adequately investigate the changes in the activation of the cerebellar circuits after treatment with the VWM-B program, previous neuroimaging studies have confirmed the role of the cerebellum in verbal WM, reading, balance, and complex actions [105, 106]. For example, a loop between the right VI and crus I lobules of the cerebellum and Broca's region of the left frontal lobe activates during articulatory rehearsal and verbal WM tasks [105, 107]. Activation of the right VI and crus I lobules of the cerebellum provide internal motor sequences for the phonological content of words [106]. Also, it has been reported that the loops between the bilateral cerebellar VI and VII lobules and the cerebral regions of the left inferior frontal lobe and the left inferior occipitotemporal lobe have a critical role in the reading network [108, 109]. Furthermore, researchers have recently discovered a novel topographic map in the cerebellar lobules of VI and VIIA, which shows the role of these lobules in complex motor tasks [105]. Therefore, the authors suggest considering these cerebellar regions in future neuroimaging studies by treatment with the VWM-B program.

There are limited studies that investigate balance training effects on children with DD. For example, Goulème et al. demonstrated the effect of balance training only on postural control [28]; however, Reynolds et al. reported the positive effects of the exercise‐based treatment on balance, dexterity, eye movement control, and cognitive skills underlying literacy [110]. Whereas Rack et al. [111], following a commentary on the Reynolds et al. study [110, 111], have not confirmed the reported results. Overall, there were no sufficient balance training methods to improve the balance and literacy in the children with DD. Therefore, the present study has introduced a newly designed training program, for the first time, which has positive effects concurrently on postural control and reading-related cognitive functions in children with DD. However, this study has some limitations. Various differences in the quality of educational services may be observed between different districts in Tehran as a metropolis. Regarding the socio-economic status of participants as an inclusion criterion, study participants were recruited from the public primary schools located in District 20, Tehran, Iran. Therefore, recruitment did not include students of private schools located in this region because of possible different educational services. Moreover, the present study investigated only the short-term effects of the VWM-B program, and its long-term effectiveness needs to be followed up in the future. We suggest investigating the effectiveness of the VWM-B program on attention using suitable measurements such as eye-tracking studies to investigate visual attention [112] and eye-movement changes, especially fixation [113] as an indicator for improving attention in DD. Also for future studies, a dyslexia control group without interventions should be considered.

Conclusions

The present study is a pioneer in investigating the effectiveness of a newly structured VWM-B training program. This program provides a dual-task condition including cognitive (verbal WM and reading) and motor (passive and active balance state) tasks. This study demonstrated that the VWM-B program, after the short-term treatment, is more effective than the VWM-program in the improvement of verbal WM capacity, reading skills, and postural control in the children with DD. The improvement in postural control (automatization in the balance-related movements) probably had an effective role in improving the measured cognitive functions. It seems that the automatization in balance-related movements consequently led to assigning further neural resources to the cognitive task. The Cerebellum has a critical role in maintaining postural control and automatizing skills; therefore, the activation of the cerebellum regions may be changed after the intervention by the VWM-B program. Despite the results of the current studies that support the cerebellar deficits hypothesis, the role of the cerebellum in DD is still controversial.

Availability of data and materials

The data analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

WM:

Working memory

VWM-B:

Verbal working memory-balance

WISC-IV:

Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition

CSI-4:

Child symptoms inventory-four

CoP:

Center of pressure

NEMA:

Persian battery of normative reading tests

WR:

Words reading

NWR:

Non-words reading

FDS:

Forward digit span

References

  1. Ashburn SM, Flowers DL, Napoliello EM, Eden GF. Cerebellar function in children with and without dyslexia during single word processing. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41(1):120–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Schlaggar BL, McCandliss BD. Development of neural systems for reading. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:475–503.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1301–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Boyle CA, Boulet S, Schieve LA, Cohen RA, Blumberg SJ, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Visser S, Kogan MD. Trends in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children, 1997–2008. Pediatrics. 2011;127(6):1034–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(1):2–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Snowling MJ. Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychol Res. 1981;43(2):219–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, White S, Frith U. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003;126(4):841–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Thomson JM, Richardson U, Goswami U. Phonological similarity neighborhoods and children’s short-term memory: typical development and dyslexia. Mem Cognit. 2005;33(7):1210–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Beneventi H, Tonnessen FE, Ersland L, Hugdahl K. Working memory deficit in dyslexia: behavioral and FMRI evidence. Int J Neurosci. 2010;120(1):51–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Menghini D, Finzi A, Carlesimo GA, Vicari S. Working memory impairment in children with developmental dyslexia: is it just a phonological deficity? Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36(2):199–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63(1):1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, White S, Frith U. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 4):841–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Stein J. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001;7(1):12–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Ahissar M. Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(11):458–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Valdois S, Bosse ML, Tainturier MJ. The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia. 2004;10(4):339–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ, Dean P. Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(9):508–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Developmental dyslexia, learning and the cerebellum. J Neural Transm Suppl. 2005;69:19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Berninger VW, Raskind W, Richards T, Abbott R, Stock P. A multidisciplinary approach to understanding developmental dyslexia within working-memory architecture: genotypes, phenotypes, brain, and instruction. Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33(6):707–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gathercole SE, Alloway TP, Willis C, Adams AM. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. J Exp Child Psychol. 2006;93(3):265–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wolf RC, Sambataro F, Lohr C, Steinbrink C, Martin C, Vasic N. Functional brain network abnormalities during verbal working memory performance in adolescents and young adults with dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(1):309–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ravizza SM, McCormick CA, Schlerf JE, Justus T, Ivry RB, Fiez JA. Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working memory. Brain. 2006;129(Pt 2):306–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Beneventi H, Tonnessen FE, Ersland L, Hugdahl K. Executive working memory processes in dyslexia: behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scand J Psychol. 2010;51(3):192–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Smith-Spark JH, Fisk JE. Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory. 2007;15(1):34–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Luo Y, Wang J, Wu H, Zhu D, Zhang Y. Working-memory training improves developmental dyslexia in Chinese children. Neural Regen Res. 2013;8(5):452.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Loosli SV, Buschkuehl M, Perrig WJ, Jaeggi SM. Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence. Child Neuropsychol. 2012;18(1):62–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Dahlin KIE. Effects of working memory training on reading in children with special needs. Read Writ. 2011;24(4):479–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Bucci MP, Bui-Quoc E, Gerard CL. The effect of a Stroop-like task on postural control in dyslexic children. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(10): e77920.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Goulème N, Gérard C-L, Bucci MP. The effect of training on postural control in dyslexic children. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(7): e0130196.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Gouleme N, Gerard CL, Bui-Quoc E, Bucci MP. Spatial and temporal analysis of postural control in dyslexic children. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126(7):1370–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Fawcett AJ, Nicolson RI, Dean P. Impaired performance of children with dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Ann Dyslexia. 1996;46(1):259–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Yap RL. Leij Avd: Testing the automatization deficit hypothesis of dyslexia via a dual-task paradigm. J Learn Disabil. 1994;27(10):660–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Marvel CL, Desmond JE. Functional topography of the cerebellum in verbal working memory. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20(3):271–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Bucci MP, Gerard CL, Bui-Quoc E. The effect of a cognitive task on the postural control of dyslexic children. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(11):3727–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Development of dyslexia: the delayed neural commitment framework. Front Behav Nurosci. 2019;13:112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Legrand A, Bui-Quoc E, Doré-Mazars K, Lemoine C, Gérard C-L, Bucci MP. Effect of a dual task on postural control in dyslexic children. PLoS ONE. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035301.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Wu T, Liu J, Hallett M, Zheng Z, Chan P. Cerebellum and integration of neural networks in dual-task processing. Neuroimage. 2013;65:466–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Pellecchia GL. Dual-task training reduces impact of cognitive task on postural sway. J Mot Behav. 2005;37(3):239–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Fritz NE, Cheek FM, Nichols-Larsen DS. Motor-cognitive dual-task training in persons with neurologic disorders: a systematic review. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2015;39(3):142–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Pichierri G, Murer K, de Bruin ED. A cognitive-motor intervention using a dance video game to enhance foot placement accuracy and gait under dual task conditions in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2012;12(1):74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Silsupadol P, Shumway-Cook A, Lugade V, van Donkelaar P, Chou L-S, Mayr U, Woollacott MH. Effects of single-task versus dual-task training on balance performance in older adults: a double-blind, randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehab. 2009;90(3):381–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Kim GY, Han MR, Lee HG. Effect of dual-task rehabilitative training on cognitive and motor function of stroke patients. J Phys Ther. 2014;26(1):1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Fernandes Â, Rocha N, Santos R, Tavares JMRS. Effects of dual-task training on balance and executive functions in Parkinson’s disease: a pilot study. Somatosens Mot Res. 2015;32(2):122–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Melby-Lervag M, Redick TS, Hulme C. Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2016;11(4):512–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Karbach J, Strobach T, Schubert T. Adaptive working-memory training benefits reading, but not mathematics in middle childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;21(3):285–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Redick TS, Shipstead Z, Wiemers EA, Melby-Lervag M, Hulme C. What’s working in working memory training? An educational perspective. Educ Psychol Rev. 2015;27(4):617–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Redick TS. Working memory training and interpreting interactions in intelligence interventions. Intelligence. 2015;50:14–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Melby-Lervag M, Hulme C. There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: a reply to Au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23(1):324–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Melby-Lervag M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(2):270–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(7):317–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Shipstead Z, Hicks KL, Engle RW. Cogmed working memory training: does the evidence support the claims? JARMAC. 2012;1(3):185–93.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Moradi AHM, Kormi Nouri R, Hassani J, Parhoon H. Reliability and validity of reading and dyslexia test (NEMA). Advan Cogn Sci. 2016;18(1):22–34.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Duncan LG, Johnston RS. How does phonological awareness relate to nonword reading skill amongst poor readers? Read Writ. 1999;11(5):405–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Members P. Resolution of the seven hundred and seventy session of the Higher Education Council [Translated]. Official site of the Islamic Parliament Research Center of the Islamic Republic of IRAN. 2008.

  54. Rostami R, Sadeghi V, Zarei J, Haddadi P, Mohazzab-Torabi S, Salamati P. Concurrent validity of Persian version of Wechsler intelligence scale for children-and cognitive assessment system in patients with learning disorder. Iran J Pediatr. 2013;23(2):183.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Mohammad Esmaeel E. Adaptation and standardization of child symptom inventory-4 (CSI-4). Except Child. 2007;7(1):79–96.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Sprafkin J, Volpe RJ, Gadow KD, Nolan EE, Kelly K. A DSM-IV-referenced screening instrument for preschool children: the early childhood inventory-4. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(5):604–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Achenbach TM. Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Vermont: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMC Med. 2010;8:18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Heinzel S, Schulte S, Onken J, Duong QL, Riemer TG, Heinz A, Kathmann N, Rapp MA. Working memory training improvements and gains in non-trained cognitive tasks in young and older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2014;21(2):146–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Thorell LB, Lindqvist S, Bergman Nutley S, Bohlin G, Klingberg T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev sci. 2009;12(1):106–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Pugin F, Metz A, Stauffer M, Wolf M, Jenni O, Huber R. Working memory training shows immediate and long-term effects on cognitive performance in children [version 3; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Res. 2015. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.3665.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Julious S. The size of a pilot study for a clinical trial should be calculated in relation to considerations of precision and efficiency. Pharm Stat. 2005;4:287–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Kim J, Shin W. How to do random allocation (randomization). Clin Orthop Surg. 2014;6(1):103–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Shokouhi S, Abdolahi E, Behzadipour S. Validity of a customized balance board for assessment of static and dynamic balance. In: 27th annual conference of mechanical engineering. Iranian Sociaty of Mechanical Engineers. 2019.

  65. Pourghayoomi E, Behzadipour S, Ramezani M, Joghataei MT, Shahidi GA. A new postural stability-indicator to predict the level of fear of falling in Parkinson’s disease patients. Biomed Eng Online. 2020;19(1):64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Woods DL, Kishiyamaa MM, Lund EW, Herron TJ, Edwards B, Poliva O, Hink RF, Reed B. Improving digit span assessment of short-term verbal memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2011;33(1):101–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Orangi M, Atefvahid MK, Ashayeri H. Standardization of the Revised Wechsler memory scale in Shiraz. IJPCP. 2002;7(4):56–66.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Homack S, Riccio CA. A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop color and word test with children. Arch Clin Nuropsych. 2004;19(6):725–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Lazarus PJ, Ludwig RP, Aberson B. Stroop color-word test: a screening measure of selective attention to differentiate LD from non LD children. J Educ Psychol. 1984;21(1):53–60.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Golden ZL, Golden CJ. Patterns of performance on the Stroop color and word test in children with learning, attentional, and psychiatric disabilities. J Sch Psychol. 2002;39(5):489–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Fischer JS. Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Pourghayoomi E, Negahdar F, Shahidi G, Hassani Mehraban A, Ebrahimi I, Taghizade G, Mirshoja M, Saeidi Boroujeni M. Correlation between functional balance and mobility tests and postural sway measures in dual task paradigm in Parkinson’s disease (a pilot study). JBCPP. 2014;2(2):1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Loras H, Sigmundsson H, Stensdotter A-K, Talcott JB. Postural control is not systematically related to reading skills: implications for the assessment of balance as a risk factor for developmental dyslexia. PloS ONE. 2014;9(6):e98224–e98224.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Pozzo T, Vernet P, Creuzot-Garcher C, Robichon F, Bron A, Quercia P. Static postural control in children with developmental dyslexia. Neurosci Lett. 2006;403(3):211–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Polskaia N, Lajoie Y. Reducing postural sway by concurrently performing challenging cognitive tasks. Hum Mov Sci. 2016;46:177–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Pia Bucci M, Bui-Quoc E, Gerard C-L. The effect of a Stroop-like task on postural control in dyslexic children. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(10): e77920.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  77. Gouglidis V, Nikodelis T, Hatzitaki V, Amiridis IG. Changes in the limits of stability induced by weight-shifting training in elderly women. Exp Aging Res. 2011;37(1):46–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Ghasemi A, Zahediasl S. Normality tests for statistical analysis: a guide for non-statisticians. Int J Endocrinol Metab. 2012;10(2):486–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Field A. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. New York: Sage; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Lakens D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol. 2013;4:863–863.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Richardson JT. Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ Res Rev. 2011;6(2):135–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Shatil E. Does combined cognitive training and physical activity training enhance cognitive abilities more than either alone? A four-condition randomized controlled trial among healthy older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5: 8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Laatar R, Kachouri H, Borji R, Rebai H, Sahli S. Combined physical-cognitive training enhances postural performances during daily life tasks in older adults. Exp Gerontol. 2018;107:91–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Schmidt M, Mavilidi MF, Singh A, Englert C. Combining physical and cognitive training to improve kindergarten children’s executive functions: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2020;63: 101908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Fraizer EV, Mitra S. Methodological and interpretive issues in posture-cognition dual-tasking in upright stance. Gait Posture. 2008;27(2):271–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Veltman DJ, Rombouts SARB, Dolan RJ. Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2003;18(2):247–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Procedural learning, dyslexia and delayed neural commitment. In: Lachmann T, Weis T, editors. Reading and dyslexia. Springer: Berlin; 2018. p. 235–69.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  88. Barela JA, Dias JL, Godoi D, Viana AR, de Freitas PB. Postural control and automaticity in dyslexic children: the relationship between visual information and body sway. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32(5):1814–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Huxhold O, Li S-C, Schmiedek F, Lindenberger U. Dual-tasking postural control: aging and the effects of cognitive demand in conjunction with focus of attention. Brain Res Bull. 2006;69(3):294–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Polskaia N, Richer N, Dionne E, Lajoie Y. Continuous cognitive task promotes greater postural stability than an internal or external focus of attention. Gait Posture. 2015;41(2):454–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Isableu B, Hlavackova P, Diot B, Vuillerme N. Regularity of center of pressure trajectories in expert gymnasts during bipedal closed-eyes quiet standing. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:317.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Rochelle KS, Talcott JB. Impaired balance in developmental dyslexia? A meta-analysis of the contending evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(11):1159–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Stoodley CJ, Stein JF. Cerebellar function in developmental dyslexia. Cerebellum. 2013;12(2):267–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Loras H, Sigmundsson H, Stensdotter A-K, Talcott JB. Postural control is not systematically related to reading skills: implications for the assessment of balance as a risk factor for developmental dyslexia. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(6): e98224.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  95. Wang S, Gathercole SE. Working memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: memory span and dual task coordination. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;115(1):188–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Knoop-van Campen CAN, Segers E, Verhoeven L. How phonological awareness mediates the relation between working memory and word reading efficiency in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2018;24(2):156–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Rispens J, Baker A. Nonword repetition: the relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and reading impairment. J Speech Lang Hear. 2012;55(3):683–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Tong X, Deacon SH, Kirby JR, Cain K, Parrila R. Morphological awareness: a key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. J Educ Psychol. 2011;103(3):523–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Soutschek A, Strobach T, Schubert T. Working memory demands modulate cognitive control in the Stroop paradigm. Psychol Res. 2013;77(3):333–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Kalanthroff E, Avnit A, Henik A, Davelaar EJ, Usher M. Stroop proactive control and task conflict are modulated by concurrent working memory load. Psychon Bull Rev. 2015;22(3):869–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Fujita H, Kasubuchi K, Osumi M, Morioka S. Effects of the central executive on postural control. J Mot Behav. 2016;48(3):270–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Oberauer K. Working memory and attention—a conceptual analysis and review. J Cogn. 2019. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Procedural learning difficulties: reuniting the developmental disorders? Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(4):135–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Nicolson RI, Fawcett A, Brookes R, Needle J. Procedural learning and dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2010;16(3):194–212.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Schlerf JE, Verstynen TD, Ivry RB, Spencer RMC. Evidence of a novel somatopic map in the human neocerebellum during complex actions. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(6):3330–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Marien P, Ackermann H, Adamaszek M, Barwood CH, Beaton A, Desmond J, De Witte E, Fawcett AJ, Hertrich I, Kuper M, et al. Consensus paper: language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum. 2014;13(3):386–410.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Chen SH, Desmond JE. Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. Neuroimage. 2005;24(2):332–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Stoodley CJ. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum. 2012;11(2):352–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Carreiras M, Mechelli A, Estevez A, Price CJ. Brain activation for lexical decision and reading aloud: two sides of the same coin? J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(3):433–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Reynolds D, Nicolson RI, Hambly H. Evaluation of an exercise-based treatment for children with reading difficulties. Dyslexia. 2003;9(1):48–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Rack JP, Snowling MJ, Hulme C, Gibbs S. No evidence that an exercise-based treatment programme (DDAT) has specific benefits for children with reading difficulties. Dyslexia. 2007;13(2):97–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Al-Wabil A, Zaphiris P, Wilson S. Examining visual attention of dyslexics on web navigation structures with eye tracking. In: 2008 international conference on innovations in information technology: 2008: IEEE; 2008. p. 717–21.

  113. Skottun BC, Skoyles JR. Attention, reading and dyslexia. Clin Exp Optom. 2006;89(4):241–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participating families, children, assessors, and education office staff of District 20, Tehran, Iran. Special thanks to Djawad Movafaghian Research Center in Rehabilitation Technologies (DMRCINT), Sharif University of Technology for experimental setup and technical supporting. This research was partially supported by funding from Iran National Science Foundation (INSF) Grant No. 92042014.

Funding

This work was supported by the Iran University of Medical Sciences (Grant Numbers 96-03-87-31977). Authors have used the grant for staff's salaries (for data collection and analysis) and traveling costs. The founder was not involved in the design, method, data collection and analysis, interpretation of data, and writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MR performed the VWM and VWM-B programs design, study design, subject recruitment, data collection, data analysis and interpretation, and initial manuscript preparation. ES, SB, and MTJ coordinated the study. EP involved in data analysis and results critique. AF participated in the study design. MR, EP, AF, and SB participated in the manuscript revision. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elham Shirazi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Before participation in the study, all parents were notified about the purposes of the research, the study procedures (e.g., random assignment to control and intervention groups), the potential consequences of the study, their right to accept or refuse research participation, their responsibilities as the parents of a child participating in research, and the responsibilities of investigators to parents and children. All parents provided informed written consent before their child’s participation in the study, and all children were compensated for their participation. The current study followed the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol of the current research, as a part of a Ph.D. thesis, had been approved by the Research Ethics Committee, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran (IR.IUMS.REC1396.31977). Also, the trial was (retrospectively) registered on 8 February 2018 with the Iranian Registry of Clinical Trials (IRCT20171219037953N1).

Consent for publication

The image of a child in Fig. 2 has been fully anonymized, and the parent provided informed written consent for using their child’s image in the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ramezani, M., Behzadipour, S., Pourghayoomi, E. et al. Evaluating a new verbal working memory-balance program: a double-blind, randomized controlled trial study on Iranian children with dyslexia. BMC Neurosci 22, 55 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-021-00660-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-021-00660-1

Keywords