Skip to main content

Neural correlates of free recall of “famous events” in a “hypermnestic” individual as compared to an age- and education-matched reference group

Abstract

Background

Memory performance of an individual (within the age range: 50–55 years old) showing superior memory abilities (protagonist PR) was compared to an age- and education-matched reference group in a historical facts (“famous events”) retrieval task.

Results

Contrasting task versus baseline performance both PR and the reference group showed fMRI activation patterns in parietal and occipital brain regions. The reference group additionally demonstrated activation patterns in cingulate gyrus, whereas PR showed additional widespread activation patterns comprising frontal and cerebellar brain regions. The direct comparison between PR and the reference group revealed larger fMRI contrasts for PR in right frontal, superior temporal and cerebellar brain regions.

Conclusions

It was concluded that PR generally recruits brain regions as normal memory performers do, but in a more elaborate way, and furthermore, that he applied a memory-strategy that potentially includes executively driven multi-modal transcoding of information and recruitment of implicit memory resources.

Background

Aside from the time-based distinction into short-term/working and long-term memory [1], memory is nowadays partitioned into content-based systems [2, 3]. Long term memory (LTM) has been discussed to be distinguished into episodic-autobiographical memory (memory for personal events or experiences), semantic memory (conscious knowledge of facts, including factual self-knowledge), perceptual memory (conscious familiarity judgments), procedural memory (mechanical, motor-related skills) and priming (higher likelihood of re-identifying previously perceived stimuli) [4,5,6]. Superior LTM performance has been described to tap into different memory systems. There are reports of individuals with highly superior autobiographical memory [7, 8], of semantic memory experts [9,10,11], and descriptions of specific types of hypermnesia [12, 13].

In general, LTM performance might be facilitated by the application of optimal learning strategies [14, 15] and/or the existence of elaborated, domain-specific expert knowledge networks [16], to which new information can be both efficiently associated to and recalled from. Parker et al. [7] proposed a specific form of superior memory performance, the hyperthymestic syndrome (HS), which is characterized by superior memory that is assumed to be automatically organized, and not based on explicit mnemonic strategies. It addresses idiosyncratic memory domains; these particular individuals do not necessarily score higher than average on standard memory tests tapping on information that is irrelevant for them. In a similar way, Norman Brown [17] put forth a model of “historical memory” elaboration that assumes an association of historical facts of public interest to individually relevant episodic information, which might be related to both the template theory proposed by Gobet and Clarkson [16] and, in case of superior performance, to the HS advanced by Parker et al. [7].

The neural mechanisms for memory encoding and retrieval are still under debate, and especially for superior memory performance, they are still largely unknown. In particular, since the prominent patient H.M. showed selective impairment in consciously encoding and consolidating new facts and events long-term [18,19,20], these processes were assumed to be mediated by the hippocampus and its adjacent medial temporal brain regions [21,22,23]. However, other regions of the limbic system situated in medial diencephalon and basal forebrain, equally contribute to these processes [5]. There are also theories suggesting that LTM networks can be associated with highly integrated networks distributed all over the neural system [24,25,26] and that the hippocampal formation is involved in both conscious and unconscious information processing [27, 28].

Functional neuroimaging and clinical studies involving patients with different forms of amnesia as a result of regional brain damage, however, support the idea that different memory systems might recruit at least partially “distinct” brain networks [5, 20, 29]. Aside from limbic structures involved in processes of binding and associating information to LTM, further cortical structures—sometimes referred to as expanded or greater limbic system [30, 31]—such as the retrosplenial cortex and the precuneus were related to processes of imagination, of the representation of memories, and to familiarity [32,33,34]. These structures might play an important role in both normal, but in particular, in superior LTM performance.

In the present study, we carried out a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment and additionally conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study that focussed on a memory retrieval task in an individual (PR) with superior historical facts knowledge (“famous events memory”). The present experimental design was specifically designed to examine the neural correlates of recalling memories related to historical facts. FMRI-contrast included the conditions ‘recall of declarative memory content WITH reference to historical facts’ and ‘recall of declarative memory content WITHOUT reference to historical facts’. It was assumed that PR should show less pronounced activation patterns in frontal (i.e., executive memory organization), but enhanced neural involvement of limbic brain regions (automatic and/or pre-attentive memory organisation) and precuneus (perceptual, imaginative, confidence judgments and/or familiarity based memory strategies) in comparison to an age- and education-matched reference group.

Results

Behavioural data

Before applying parametric t-statistics, a Shapiro–Wilk-test (S–W-T) was performed in order to test, whether variables of interest were normal-distributed. In the reference group, the percentage of freely recalled correct answers (consistently shown for both scanner session and post hoc debriefing) was significantly higher in the BASE (test on normal distribution: S–W-T, W = .93, p = .45) compared to the TASK (test on normal distribution: S–W-T, W = .88, p = .13)-condition (TASK: 41.7 ± 8.3%; BASE: 89.6 ± 7.2%; t Test: t = 15.2, p < .001; see Fig. 1a), and reference group members performed better than chance level in both TASK- and BASE-conditions (TASK vs. 25%: t = − 1.9, p < .05; BASE vs. 25%: t = − 8.6, p < .001 [35, 36]). Compared to the reference group, PR showed a higher correct percentage rate in the TASK-condition (t = 4.1, p < .01 [35, 36]; see Fig. 1a) and comparable performance in the BASE-condition. All participants showed high performance in the BASE task. As, however, performance-values were normal-distributed (see above) and PR's performance ranged in the middle of the reference-group performances, we rather tend to disclose a ceiling effect. Participants in the reference group showed longer response times for correctly answered TASK-trials (test on normal distribution: S–W-T, W = .87, p = .09) compared to BASE-trials (test on normal distribution: S–W-T, W = .90, p = .24) during free recall (TASK: 3728 ± 733 ms; BASE: 3056 ± 429 ms; t = 4.4, p < .01; see Fig. 1b). According to Crawford and Howell [35], PR (TASK: 3599 ms; BASE: 3664 ms) did not differ in any response time value from the reference group.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Memory performance in percentage of correct freely recalled answers (left panel) and the respective mean response times (right panel); asterisks indicate significant differences (p < .05; details, see text) and whiskers indicate standard deviations

FMRI-data: TASK versus BASE-condition in PR and in reference participants

For the reference group, second level fMRI-analysis for TASK in contrast to BASE-condition revealed activation clusters in the right anterior and left posterior cingulate, bilateral precuneus, cuneus, and lingual gyrus (Fig. 2a; Table 1, column A). Protagonist PR showed activation clusters in widespread bilateral superior, medial, and middle frontal gyri, right postcentral gyrus, left precuneus, right middle and left inferior occipital gyri, left fusiform gyrus, and left cerebellar regions for the contrast TASK versus BASE-condition (Fig. 2b; Table 1, column B).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Glass-brain views of the contrasts TASK versus BASE for (a) the reference group, (b) the protagonist PR, (c) protagonist PR versus reference group and (d) vice versa (details for statistical procedures, see methods section). The respective anatomical regions, MNI to Talairach transformed coordinates and t-values were listed in Table 1 (columns A–D). All contrasts: p < .001

Table 1 Anatomical regions, peak activation t-values, and Talairach-coordinates for contrasts between TASK- and BASE-conditions (arranged in columns A–D) separately for (column A) the reference group (RG, one-sample t-test), (column B) the protagonist (PR, first level contrast), (column C) PR versus RG, and (column D) RG versus PR (latter two comparisons according to Crawford and Garthwaite [42, 43]

All PSC-value-distributions were tested for deviations from normal distribution via Shapiro–Wilk-Test: rPCG: W = .92, p = .33; rSFG: W = .86, p = .05; lMFG: W = .98, p = .96; first cluster rMFG: W = .94, p = .48; second cluster rMFG: .93, p = .42; rSTG: W = .94, p = .47; rCUL: W = .92, p = .29. The direct comparison between PR and reference group yielded larger contrasts between TASK- and BASE-conditions distributed over precentral gyrus, right superior and bilateral middle frontal gyri, right superior temporal gyrus, and the left culmen (Fig. 2c; Table 1, column C). Reference group versus PR showed larger contrasts in left paracentral lobule, right precuneus, and left cuneus (Fig. 2d; Table 1, column D).

In Fig. 3, distributions of difference-values between PSC-values for TASK and BASE-conditions for several ROIs were illustrated for the reference group and separately for PR. T-Test for single means yielded to individual versus group differences in right precentral gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus, left and right middle frontal gyri, and right superior temporal gyrus.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Regions of interest (ROI-) analyses showing section views of seven ROIs. Box-plots illustrate the distribution of percent signal change (PSC) difference values (TASK—BASE) for the reference group and black dots indicate the respective values for protagonist PR; rPCG = right PreCentral Gyrus, rSFG = right Superior Frontal Gyrus; lMFG = left Middle Frontal Gyrus; rMFG = right Middle Frontal Gyrus; rSTG = right Superior Temporal Gyrus; rCUL = right CULmen

Discussion

In the present study, functional neuroimaging was used to compare a protagonist’s (PR) superior memory performance in a famous events free retrieval task (contrasted with a semantic non-historical facts free retrieval task serving as baseline) with the performance of a reference group. PR performed significantly better than the reference group and showed activation patterns predominantly distributed over frontal and cerebellar, but also in parietal, occipital and occipito-temporal brain regions. The reference group demonstrated activation patterns in the cingulate cortex, parietal, and occipital brain regions. The direct comparison between PR and the reference group confirmed larger contrasts for frontal and cerebellar regions in PR and for parietal and occipital brain areas in the reference group. It appears that PR predominantly recruited right hemispheric frontal resources potentially related to his superior memory retrieval performance [44, 45].

Which type of expert is PR?

First, PR showed superior memory performance for famous events retrieval, and he scored predominantly at average or above average in most retrograde and anterograde memory tests in the neuropsychological assessment. Only for complex anterograde non-verbal memory processing he scored below average. Incidentally, the case described by Parker et al. [7] also showed impairments in anterograde non-verbal memory tasks. Thus, while PR shares certain similitudes with HS (hyperthymestic syndrome [7]), he presents with unique features, which deviate from the HS prototype. He showed, beside superior historical facts memory, deviations from standards in non-idiosyncratically eminent memory domains. The mnemonic performance of PR can be at least partly understood within the model of Markowitsch and Tulving. Incidentally, Tulving [46] indirectly anticipated later models of mnemonic processing, which describe porous boundaries between memory systems [47]. In 1995, Tulving [46] proposed his SPI-model which states that encoding of information follows a regular sequence—that means it is serial in that way that first simple, implicitly functioning, memory systems are engaged and only at the end of the series explicit, episodic encoding occurs. Information then is stored in parallel memory systems in the brain and it can be retrieved independently from the systems used for the encoding process (SPI = Serial, Parallel, Independent). As PR describes his knowledge as “popping out automatically”, it can be speculated that according to Tulving’s [46] SPI-model, PR retrieves his knowledge “automatically”, and therefore more independently from his initial encoding of it, than it likely is the case in most human beings.

In addition, PR showed sub-average performance on tests for executive functions. While this type of performance may be interpreted as a hint towards a savant-syndrome [48], there was no clear clinical, neuropsychological or anamnestic evidence in his case that he fell into this category. He showed a superior intelligence level comparable to the reference group as confirmed by a verbal intelligence test. Furthermore, he scored above average on standardized laboratory tests for assessing social cognition and emotional processing, although he reported some interpersonal difficulties in real-life, which were being addressed in psychotherapy. Incidentally, the case described by Parker et al. [7] also presented with some difficulties in the executive functioning domain. Furthermore, LePort et al. [8] found in a case series of patients with highly superior autobiographical memory abilities a psychological profile indicative of obsessive compulsive tendencies. These tendencies typically are accompanied by a reduction in cognitive flexibility, which gets translated in impaired performance on corresponding neuropsychological tasks. The deficient performance of PR on tasks tapping on cognitive flexibility and the anamnestic reports about PR may speak in favour of obsessive compulsive personality phenotypic traits. These traits may promote an automatic engagement in repetition of mnemonic material of special interest, with consequences for processes of encoding and consolidation. Conclusively, PR cannot be seen comparable to individuals with HS or to savants in the classical sense. It rather appears that he is an expert sharing some features of HS and savant people, but does not fully overlap with any of these prototypes, displaying unique features. In the following, characteristics in fMRI activation patterns are discussed to conclude about the individual mnemonic strategy that PR might have applied to score higher in free historical facts retrieval than the reference group.

Can the present functional neuroimaging data explain PR´s profile of superior memory performance?

The hypothesis that PR should show rather posterior and/or subcortical instead of frontal activation patterns when compared to the reference group, had to be rejected. The opposite was the case: PR showed predominantly right frontal patterns of larger contrasts between free historical and non-historical semantic facts retrieval. At first glance, this finding appears quite disillusioning in view of the hypotheses postulated in the introduction section, however, combined with the data that PR also showed remarkable cerebellar recruitment, a hybrid mental strategy of memory processing including both explicit and implicit components might be conceivable [10, 48]. And, this argument also goes with the hybrid classification (between savant and expert) concluded from the neuropsychological testing data as mentioned in the previous section.

Level of processing and multi-modal integration of memory

There were also exclusive activation patterns in PR not present in the reference group and vice versa, which could in part only be interpreted in an individual (PR) or group-internal (reference-group) way as the direct comparison between PR and the reference group did not reach statistical significance in the respective brain regions. In posterior brain regions, associated with perceptual information processing, PR showed activation patterns rather adjacent to primary (i.e., middle and inferior occipital gyrus, and postcentral gyrus) and less in hetero-modal, medial (i.e., cuneus and lingual gyrus) cortical brain regions as the reference group did. Rather medial activation patterns in the reference group might be related to a higher level complex or abstract memory processing [25]. This finding might also explain why PR scored above average in simple, but below average in complex recognition tasks that usually require a deeper associative memory strategy.

Transcoding and integration of information across processing modalities (i.e., implicit and explicit processing, and verbal, visual, and spatial modalities, etc.), as can be seen for example in synesthesia [49,50,51] was discussed as a potential feature of both superior expert and savant memory performance involving occipito-temporal regions such as the fusiform gyrus [9, 10]. The present data showed exclusive fusiform gyrus involvement in PR, but not in the reference group. It should however be mentioned that the direct comparison between groups via between-group t-test did not reveal differences in this region. Nevertheless, in combination with an exclusive cerebellar recruitment in PR, this data might point to an implicit-explicit (and vice versa) memory transcoding strategy facilitating superior memory performance in historical facts retrieval, but also in other simple memory processing domains as shown by the neuropsychological assessment.

The stronger involvement of superior temporal gyrus (STG) in PR additionally supports the idea of pronounced multi-modal integration of information in LTM potentially facilitating his retrieval performance. The multi-associative nature of STG has been documented by studies discussing the role of the STG in retrieving both autobiographical events [52,53,54] and semantic facts, such as public events or “long-established knowledge about the world” [52, 55, 56].

Furthermore, the right superior temporal cortex was thought to be engaged in spatial awareness and exploration [57]. Along this line of argumentation, the stronger recruitment of STG in PR may signify a strategy to navigate through personal past by making use of more elaborated spatial exploration strategies [53]. Recently, Manning and colleagues argued that public semantic memory is supported by both the semantic and episodic memory system [58]. The stronger involvement of the STG in PR in contrast to the reference group might again point to a particular engagement of the episodic memory system in public semantic knowledge processing in the case of PR and other individuals with highly superior autobiographical talents [8, 59,60,61].

Limitations of the present study

In future studies the number of tasks in the different task-condition should be perfectly matched.

For the present reference group there were 15–25 correct response trials in the TASK-condition left to be modeled for the respective fMRI-statistics. Despite normal distribution and despite numerous studies also reporting reliable data based on a quite low number of trials and/or individuals, this point should be carefully considered for an appropriate interpretation of the here presented data. In future studies additional measurement sessions should be taken into account to extend the number of valid trials. In the present study, we were however forced to balance the available time (PR was only available for a short time period) and test-statistical requirements.

Conclusions

Specific complex mental processes cannot be inferred directly from functional brain imaging data [62], however, there is evidence that regional brain activation can help to understand the underlying mental principles involved in a certain complex mental process such as the applied visual, verbal or spatial modality, or perceptual and/or executive processing types, and others more [63,64,65]. It appears that the utilization of individual mental strategies also plays an important role in effective memory processing [61, 66,67,68,69]. And, these individual memory strategies can be modulated by executive mental processing as potentially reflected in the pronounced recruitment of frontal brain regions in PR. Furthermore, the present data support the idea that superior mental processing in experts can be facilitated by the conceptually driven recruitment of implicit/procedural memory resources (i.e., potentially reflected by the involvement of cerebellar brain areas [10]). A more detailed assessment of mental strategies in individuals with superior mental performance can provide insights into effective implicit memory usage potentially driven by explicit executive mental processing. Functional neuroimaging can help to evaluate the recruitment of implicit mental resources that are difficult to be assessed by explicit surveys.

Methods

Study participants

The individual protagonist PR (within the age range: 50–55 years old) was a healthy, right handed expert with superior memory abilities.

Applying a test-battery several months before the fMRI-measurement session provided a detailed neuropsychological performance-profile of PR (see Table 2 for details).

Table 2 Protagonist PR was examined with a battery of different test inventories

The reference group consisted of 10 male adults between 47 and 62 years (54.6 ± 4.3 years.; not differing in age from PR: t = − .8, p = .445; [35, 36]. All participants were right handed according to a modified version of the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire [37], and did not report psychiatric or neurological illness or psychotropic drug treatment. All participants were native German speakers holding a university degree. The participants were familiarized with the assessment environment and their participation was solely motivated by their interest in scientific investigations.

After the fMRI-session, verbal intelligence was examined in all participants with the MWT-B (Mehrfach–Wahl–Wortschatz-Test [38]), for which PR reached 130, and members of the reference group reached 126.0 ± 13.6 (test on normal distribution with Shapiro-Wilk-test: W = .89, p = .18). Here, PR did not differ from the group (t = − .28, p = .79 [35, 36]).

The study protocol was designed and performed according to the Helsinki Declaration (1964) and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Bremen University. All participants were informed about the procedure, and gave written consent to participate in the experiment.

Task and stimuli

The tasks were presented visually. The experimental set-up includes two task conditions, a baseline (BASE) and a task of interest (TASK). Both task conditions included questions in order to test semantic memory performance. BASE-condition tested semantic memory about common knowledge such as for example “How is the head of the Catholic church called?”, and the TASK-condition tested semantic memory about public historical facts from contemporary history, for example “Which city hosted the Olympic Summer Games in 1996?”.

The respective questions were presented via a digital projector on a mirror in the scanner tube in the center of the display as a centered text-block (see Fig. 4 for illustration). There was no time limit to think about the correct answer. Responses consisted of pressing the answer button with the right index finger. Individuals, however, were encouraged not to ruminate too long about the answer and they were asked to press a button with the right index finger, if they either believed to know the correct answer or when they were sure not to know the answer. This procedure was applied to ensure that participants produced free recall and not recognition performance. After pressing the button, four alternative answers (one correct solution) were presented slightly shifted one below the other in the center of the display (see Fig. 4 for illustration). If participants freely recalled the correct answer, they were asked to choose it from the four alternatives. If however, they did not recall the correct answer, but recognized it among the four alternatives, they were allowed to choose the correct one. If they neither recalled nor recognized the correct answer, participants were asked to wait (ten seconds) for the next trial. Between trials a fixation dot was presented for a pseudo-randomly jittered interval ranging between 3000 and 4000 ms.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Experimental trial and trial elements: (1) Question, waiting for button press after free recall or conformation of omission, (2) choosing the correct answer after recall or recognition, or wait ten seconds for the between-trial period, and (3) between-trial fixation-period (3500 ± 500 ms, pseudo-randomised jittered)

After the MRI-scanner session, all questions were again presented by a paper–pencil-test. The participants were asked again to answer correctly to all BASE- and TASK-questions and to further provide detailed information about whether they freely recalled, recognized or did not know the correct answers during the fMRI-measurement. BASE- and TASK-trials were only included in behavioural and fMRI-data-analyses, if they were consistently answered correctly during both the fMRI-measurement and the paper–pencil-test, and furthermore, if they were labeled to be freely recalled and not just recognized. All other trials were neglected in behavioural data analyses and modeled as dummy-trials in the fMRI-analyses (see below).

50 BASE- and 48 TASK-trials were presented in a pseudo-randomized non-stationary probabilistic weighted sequence [39] during one experimental run of 17 ± 2 min.

FMRI data acquisition and analyses

FMRI-data acquisition

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were collected on a SIEMENS MAGNETOM scanner (Skyra syngo MR D13, 3 Tesla). The images were acquired using a BOLD weighted gradient echo echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence (TE 30 ms). Forty four slices were acquired in interleaved order with no gap in axial orientation parallel to AC-PC with a GRAPPA accelleration factor of two leading to a TR of 2500 ms. The image volume covered the entire cerebrum and cerebellum. The in plane resolution was 3 × 3 mm2, the corresponding matrix  64 × 64, 411 ± 44 volumes were obtained during the functional run. Structural whole head T1 weighted images were acquired (TR/TE/TI/flip angle = 2400 ms/2.43 ms/900 ms/8°; matrix 256 × 256; slice thickness 1.0 mm; FOV 256 mm; 176 slices) for all participants.

FMRI-data analysis

Image analysis was performed using additional algorithms for the comparison of single individuals with group-related data (see below), which were implemented in SPM (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). For each session and participant, images were realigned to the first image in the time series to correct for head motion. The realigned images were spatially normalized into a standard stereotaxic space (Montreal Neurological Institute template) using a 12 parameter affine model. Dimensions after normalising procedures were 79(x) × 95(y) × 69(z) and resulting voxel size was 2 mm3. These spatially normalized images were smoothed to minimize noise and residual differences in gyral anatomy with a Gaussian filter set at 8.0 mm. Prior to the statistical analysis, a temporal high pass filter (250 s) was applied and global effects were removed. Pre-processed data sets were analysed using second-level random effects models [40] on the individual parameter estimates.

FMRI data were modelled for different trial element phases (see Fig. 4 for illustration): (1) from the start of task presentation until the first button press (separately for correct, incorrect, and omitted trials), (2) from the display of the four response alternatives to the second button press or until the trial time runs out (separately for correct, incorrect, and omitted trials), and (3) the fixation dot period between task trials resulting in 13 regressors in the design matrices. At the single-individual level, a t-contrast at each voxel for each participant was computed to produce statistical images for the contrast TASK- versus BASE-condition for the free recall trial element. At the second level, the resulting contrast images were used to identify the main task effect TASK versus BASE-condition by means of a one sample t-test (p < .001, uncorrected). For PR, TASK versus BASE-conditions were contrasted using t-statistics (p < .001, uncorrected). Percent signal change values for several regions of interest (ROIs) were extracted applying the software package Marsbar (Version 4.2 [41]). ROIs were extracted according to activation clusters resulting from the contrast “PR vs. reference group” related to “TASK vs. BASE”-condition contrasts. This was done to comprehensibly illustrate core aspects of the present data.

In order to inferentially compare brain activation patterns between PR and the reference group, we decided to follow the methods suggested by Crawford and colleagues [35, 42, 43]. The respective algorithms were implemented as an SPM compliant function that reads the specified individual contrast images and their respective design matrices (SPM.mat files). The appropriate beta images are then loaded as scores for the respective tasks and the calculus is performed. The resulting images were written to disk as spmT image files for use in the result function of SPM (p < .001, uncorrected) (see Fehr et al. [10] for further methodological details).

Abbreviations

FMRI:

functional magnetic resonance imaging

HS:

hyperthymestic syndrome

LTM:

long term memory

PR:

protagonist

SPM:

statistical parametric mapping

S–W-T:

Shapiro–Wilk-test

References

  1. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Tulving E. Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human? In: Terrace HS, Metcalfe J, editors. The missing link in cognition: Self-knowing consciousness in man and animals. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 3–56.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Markowitsch HJ. Psychogenic amnesia. Neuroimage. 2003;20:132–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Markowitsch HJ, Staniloiu A. Amnestic disorders. Lancet. 2012;380:1429–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(11)61304-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Staniloiu A, Markowitsch HJ. Dissociative amnesia. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1:226–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Parker ES, Cahill L, McGaugh JL. A case of unusual autobiographical remembering. Neurocase. 2006;12:35–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. LePort AKR, Mattfeld AT, Dickinson-Anson H, Fallon JH, Craig EL, Stark CEL, Kruggel F, Cahill L, McGaugh JL. Behavioral and neuroanatomical investigation of Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM). Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2012;98:78–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Amidzic O, Riehle HJ, Fehr T, Elbert T. Focal gamma band activity: the signature of chunks in the expert memory of chess players. Nature. 2001;412:603.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Fehr T, Weber J, Willmes K, Herrmann M. Neural correlates in exceptional mental arithmetic—About the neural architecture of prodigious skills. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1407–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Fehr T, Wallace G, Erhard P, Herrmann M. The functional neuroanatomy of expert calendar calculation: a matter of strategy? Neurocase. 2011;17:360–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Erdelyi MH, Becker J. Hypermnesia for pictures: incremental memory for pictures but not for words in multiple recall trials. Cogn Psychol. 1974;6:159–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Bluck S, Levine LJ, Laulhere TM. Autobiographical remembering and hypermnesia: a comparison of older and younger adults. Psychol Aging. 1999;14:671–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Bor D, Duncan J, Wisemann RJ, Owen AM. Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron. 2003;37:361–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Kuo MCC, Liu KPY, Chan CCH. Factors involved in memory encoding and their implications for the memory performance of older adults and people with mild cognitive impairment. World J Neurosci. 2012;2:103–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gobet F, Clarkson G. Chunks in expert memory: evidence for the magical number four … or is it two? Memory. 2004;12:732–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Brown NR. Organization of public events in long-term memory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1990;119:297–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosur Psychiatry. 1957;20:11–21.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Squire LR. The legacy of patient H.M. for neuroscience. Neuron. 2009;61:6–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Squire LR, Wixted JT. The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:259–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Wang S-H, Morris GM. Hippocampal-neocortical interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation. Annu Rev Psychol. 2010;61:49–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Rugg MD, Vilberg KL, Mattson JT, Yu SS, Johnson JD, Suzuki M. Item memory, context memory and the hippocampus; fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:3070–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Rugg MD, Vilberg K. Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:255–60.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Fuster JM. The cognit: a network model of cortical representation. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;60:125–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Fuster JM. Cortex and memory: emergence of a new paradigm. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:2047–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Basar E. The theory of the whole-brain-work. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;60:133–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Henke K. A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:523–32.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Shohamy D, Turk-Browne NB. Mechanisms for widespread hippocampal involvement in cognition. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2013;142:1159–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Squire LR. Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82:171–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Nauta WJH. Expanding border of the limbic system concept. In: Rasmussen T, Marino R, editors. Functional neurosurgery. New York: Raven Press; 1979. p. 7–23.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Nieuwenhuys R. The greater limbic system, the emotional motor system and the brain. Prog Brain Res. 1996;107:551–80.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Shah NJ, Marshall JC, Zafiris O, Schwab A, Zilles K, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR. The neural correlates of person familiarity. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study with clinical implications. Brain. 2001;124:804–15.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ, Craik FIM, Habib R, Houle S. Novelty and familiarity activations in PET studies of memory encoding and retrieval. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:71–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Herholz K, Kessler J, Ehlen P, Lenz O, Kalbe E, Markowitsch HJ. The role of prefrontal cortex, precuneus, and cerebellum during face-name association learning. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:643–50.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Crawford JR, Howell DC. Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12:482–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1196–208.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Oldfield R. The assessment and analysis of handedness. The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest (MWT-B) [Multiple choice lexis intelligence test]. Balingen: Spitta Verlag; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Friston KJ. Experimental design and statistical issues. In: Mazziotta JC, Toga AW, editors. Brain mapping: the disorders. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000. p. 33–58.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  40. Holmes AP, Friston KJ. Generalisability, random effects, and population inference. Neuroimage. 1998;7:754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Brett M, Anton J-L, Valabregue R, Poline J-B. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract] Presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, June 2–6, 2002, Sendai, Japan. Available on CD-ROM in Neuroimage, 16, No 2; 2002.

  42. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. Testing for suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case studies in neuropsychology: evaluation of alternatives using Monte Carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations. Neuropsychology. 2005;19:318–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. Evaluation of criteria for classical dissociations in single-case studies by Monte Carlo simulation. Neuropsychology. 2005;19:664–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Tulving E, Kapur S, Markowitsch HJ, Craik G, Habib R, Houle S. Neuroanatomical correlates of retrieval in episodic memory: auditory sentence recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1994;91:2012–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Lepage M, Ghaffar O, Nyberg L, Tulving E. Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:506–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Tulving E. Organization of memory: quo vadis? In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1995. p. 839–47.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Dew ITZ, Cabeza R. The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: behavioral and neural evidence. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2011;1224:174–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Treffert DA. The savant syndrome: an extraordinary condition. A synopsis: past, present, future. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2009;364:1351–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Marks LE. On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. Psychol Bull. 1975;82:303–31.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Marks LE, Mulvenna CM. Synesthesia, at and near its borders. Front Psychol. 2013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00651.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Rouw R, Scholte HS, Colizoli O. Brain areas involved in synaesthesia: a review. J Neuropsychol. 2011;5:214–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Svoboda E, McKinnon M, Levine B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2189–208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Piefke M, Weiss PH, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR. Gender differences in the functional neuroanatomy of emotional episodic autobiographical memory. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;24:313–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Fink GR, Markowitsch HJ, Reinkemeier M, Bruckbauer T, Kessler J, Heiss W-D. Cerebral representation of one’s own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4275–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Insausti R, Annese J, Amaral DG, Squire LR. Human amnesia and the medial temporal lobe illuminated by neuropsychological and neurohistological findings for patient E.P. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:E1953–62. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1306244110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Maguire EA, Mummery CJ, Büchel C. Patterns of hippocampal–cortical interaction dissociate temporal lobe memory subsystems. Hippocampus. 2000;10:475–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Karnath HO. New insights into the functions of the superior temporal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:568–76. https://doi.org/10.1038/35086057.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Manning L, Denkova E, Unterberger L. Autobiographical significance in past and future public semantic memory: a case-study. Cortex. 2013;49:2007–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.11.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. De Renzi E, Liotti M, Nichelli P. Semantic amnesia with preservation of autobiographic memory, A case report. Cortex. 1987;23:575–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Yasuda K, Watanabe O, Ono Y. Dissociation between semantic and autobiographic memory: a case report. Cortex. 1997;33:623–38.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Maguire EA, Valentie ER, Wilding JM, Kapur N. Routes to remembering: the brains behind superior memory. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:90–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  62. Poldrack RA. Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:59–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Houdé O, Zago L, Mellet E, Moutier S, Pineau A, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Shifting from the perceptual brain to the logical brain: the neural impact of cognitive inhibition training. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:721–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Houdé O, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Neural foundations of logical and mathematical cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:507–14.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Fehr T. A hybrid model for the neural representation of complex mental processing in the human brain. Cogn Neurodyn. 2013;7:89–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Addis DR, Knapp K, Roberts RP, Schacter DL. Routes to the past: neural substrates of direct and generative autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2908–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Raz A, Packard MG, Alexander GM, Buhle JT, Zhu H, Yu S, Peterson BS. A slice of π: an exploratory neuroimaging study of digit encoding and retrieval in a superior memorist. Neurocase. 2009;15:361–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Yin L-J, Lu Y-T, Fan M-X, Wang Z-X, Hu Y. Neural evidence for the use of digit-image mnemonic in a superior memorist: an fMRI study. Front Human Neurosci. 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Patihis L, Frenda SJ, LePort AKR, Petersen N, Nichols RM, Stark CEL, McGaugh JL, Loftus EF. False memories in highly autobiographical memory individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:20947–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

TF contributed to the experimental design, data acquisition, data analyses and manuscript writing. AS contributed to the neuropsychological testing of PR and manuscript writing. HJM contributed to the experimental design and manuscript writing. PE contributed to fMRI-data acquisition and manuscript writing. MH contributed to the experimental design and manuscript writing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

Please contact the corresponding author to get local offline-access to the original data sets.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol was designed and performed according to the Helsinki Declaration (1964) and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Bremen University. All participants were informed about the procedure, and gave written consent to participate in the experiment.

Funding

Not applicable.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thorsten Fehr.

Additional file

Additional file 1.

Neural correlates of free recall of “famous events” in a “hypermnestic” individual as compared to an age- and education-matched reference group - supplementary information.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fehr, T., Staniloiu, A., Markowitsch, H.J. et al. Neural correlates of free recall of “famous events” in a “hypermnestic” individual as compared to an age- and education-matched reference group. BMC Neurosci 19, 35 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-018-0435-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-018-0435-y

Keywords