Skip to main content

Monitoring the neural activity associated with praying in Sahaja Yoga meditation

Abstract

Background

Sahaja Yoga Meditation draws on many religious traditions and uses a variety of techniques including Christian prayer to reach a state known as thoughtless awareness, or mental silence. While there are many studies on the neural correlates of meditation, few studies have focused on the neural correlates of praying. Thus, the aim of our research was to study the neural activity associated with the prayer practices in Sahaja Yoga Mediation, which have not been studied before, to explore effects beyond repetitive speech or “mantra effects”. Sixteen experienced Sahaja Yoga Meditation practitioners were scanned using task based functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging while performing formalised and improvised forms of praying and their equivalent secular tasks.

Results

Our results showed the deactivation of bilateral thalamus during both prayers compared to secular conditions and the activation in the medial prefrontal cortex that was reduced by religious and formalised secular speech conditions but increased during improvised secular speech; similarly, frontal regions were deactivated when comparing prayers to their secular equivalents.

Discussion

These results seem to depict two important factors related with praying in Sahaja Yoga Meditation merging inner concentration and social cognition. First, the perception of the surroundings mediated by the thalamus may be decreased during these prayers probably due to the establishment of inner concentration and, second, frontal deactivation effects could be related to reduced social judgement and ‘mentalizing’, particularly in the medial prefrontal cortex. Our findings suggest that praying by Sahaja Yoga Meditation practitioners is neurophenomenologically different from the social cognitive attempt of praying within Christian praying practices.

Peer Review reports

Background

Meditation comprises a wide range of different practices, most of them aiming to increase awareness, consciousness, inner peace, self-realization and attention, while reducing stress and anxiety [1]. Some meditation practices have a spiritual part which could create links with religion and some religions include meditation as part of their practices. The changes associated with meditation in the brain have boosted the interest of the neuroscientific community on this topic. Different reviews have reported differences in brain structure and function associated with the practice of meditation in cross-sectional and/or longitudinal studies [2, 3]. Sahaja Yoga Meditation (SYM) teaches its practitioners to reach a state known as thoughtless awareness or mental silence where thoughts are considerably reduced or completely vanished, which is contemplated as the utmost goal of meditation according to the ancient text “Yoga Sutras of Patanjali” [4]. In this state of mental silence, meditators are fully aware of themselves, continuously living each present moment in a state of peace, harmony and high efficiency [5].

Religion involves diverse cognitive tasks, behaviours, praying practices and spiritual beliefs which change across cultures. In most religions, an important goal is to communicate or to relate to the divine. Religion as defined by Koenig [6] is “a system of beliefs and practices observed by a community, supported by rituals that acknowledge, worship, communicate with, or approach the Sacred, the Divine, Ultimate Truth, Reality, or nirvana”. Previous studies have illustrated that religious cognition depends on contextual and developmental practices, individual goals, and sophisticated emotional and embodied states [7]. A particular example of such practices, where religion and social cognition merge, is the act of praying, which is a form of communication between the individual and God as a culturally transmitted entity [8]. Since praying is a language-based communication behaviour, which depends on the individual’s environment and experience, its structure as a ritual has become the primary subject in religious cognitive science, more specifically the difference between formalised and improvised forms of religious expression.

Research of religious cognition has revealed fascinating insights into the cognitive processes that underlie religious beliefs and practices. Unlike other beliefs, religion relies upon a mixture of cognitive and emotional functions governed by specific brain networks [9], which may be activated depending on the experimental design used in each study [10]. In their reviews on religious cognition, Rim et al. [11] and Grafman et al. [10] report findings on cognitive systems supporting religious beliefs and spirituality, including the frontal and anterior temporal lobes [12, 13]; medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), inferior frontal gyrus, temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and precuneus [8, 14]; dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) [10, 15, 16]; and vmPFC, striatum and nucleus accumbens [10, 17]. Overall, the findings suggest that religious experiences involve complex cognitive and neural processes known to affect cognitive control [18].

Several imaging studies on social cognition show the involvement of the lateral, ventral and medial prefrontal cortices, the precuneus as well as TPJ cortices during social cognition tasks [19,20,21,22]. Activity in lateral prefrontal as well as medial prefrontal and parietal areas during participants’ declarations of personal interactions with God was considered similar to that observed in social cognition tasks [23, 24]. In line with this, Schjoedt et al., [8] found activation in mPFC, the TPJ, precuneus and the temporopolar region when comparing an improvised prayer with a structured one (Lord’s Prayer). The mPFC, TPJ along with the temporopolar region have been considered as the foundational areas for theory-of-mind, meaning that these are the key areas facilitating the interpretation of beliefs and intentions of other people [19, 25]. It was then proposed by the authors that communication with God in a personal improvised prayer involves brain regions similar to those mediating interpersonal interaction involving social cognition [8].

SYM uses a large variety of religious practices among which praying plays an important role. It is common that SYM practitioners recite the Lord’s Prayer from Christianity, Psalm 23 or Psalm of David from Judaism, recite the holy names of Allāh from Islam or recite the Ganesha Atharva Sheersha from Hinduism [26, 27] as a practice to facilitate the state of mental silence meditation that has been described in previous papers [28]. In this sense, the meditative state in SYM, and its related brain activity, could be dependent on the praying practices used by SYM practitioners [29]. Therefore, brain activity associated with praying as a practice to get into meditation may help to understand different meditation practices and states.

The literature divides prayers into two different forms: highly formalised, prefixed and frequently rehearsed speech acts, and non-institutional prayers that are mainly improvised actions of speech acts [30,31,32]. This division is also reflected in SYM in which practitioners recite the Lord’s Prayer as well as praying freely to the Divine Mother. The aim of this study was therefore to explore praying in SYM beyond repetitive speech [33], known as the “mantra-effect”. We furthermore contrasted praying with similarly constructed language tasks, given the limitations of comparing praying against resting [34, 35], which as mentioned [36] is not the best control condition as does not control for inner speech related brain activation. Therefore, similarly constructed secular language tasks were included to control for language-based activations in order to isolate the effect of the prayers [29]. Thus, we used an adapted version of the study design by Schjoedt and colleagues [8, 17], to examine improvised and formalized ways of praying as preparatory practices to mental silence meditation.

The main objective of this research was therefore to determine the neural activity associated with the prayer practices within SYM to help us further characterize the basis of this meditation technique. Although SYM has been studied from different perspectives mainly focused on the brain correlates of the state of mental silence [28, 37,38,39], the neural activity associated with praying has not been scientifically covered yet, which is a key objective of this study and could provide some insights on this particular phenomenology of SYM. Since SYM practitioners direct their prayers to the divine in order to achieve the state of thoughtless awareness, we expected the involvement of similar areas as those observed in the previous study in Christian prayers [8]. Specifically, as in the study in Christians we expected the involvement of regions related to social cognition such as lateral and medial prefrontal regions, the temporo-parietal junction and the precuneus. However, we also expected the involvement of other areas we observed previously to be involved during SYM such as the anterior cingulate cortex, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), vlPFC, mPFC, insula, parietal cortex, and temporal cortex [28].

Results

Volunteers reported that they were able to concentrate and properly follow all the tasks inside the scanner despite the noise and discomfort the Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scanner generates. When asked to evaluate their performance for the different tasks from 5 “very well” to 1 “very bad”, they reported on average from well to very well, with these mean scores: Lord’s Prayer (Father) 4.5 (SD = 0.73), Prayer to the Divine Mother (Mother) 4.63 (SD = 0.62), Wishes to Santa (Santa) 4.13 (SD = 0.72), Poem 4.38 (SD = 0.62), Counting backwards 4.28 (SD = 0.82).

Main effects of Prayer act

We compared the effects on the two prayers conditions relative to their secular contrasts (Mother&Father > Santa&Poem) in order to observe the general effects of “Domain” factor, no cluster survived this contrast. For the opposite direction, which analysed the effect of higher activation during the secular contrasts compared to the religious conditions (Santa&Poem > Mother&Father) there was a strong response in four clusters including the left vlPFC/temporal pole (TP), left dlPFC/premotor cortex, bilateral thalamus and bilateral supplementary motor area (SMA)/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), see Table 1 and Fig. 1.

Table 1 Main effect of Prayer act
Fig. 1
figure 1

Main effects of secular domain (Santa&Poem > Mother&Father). A vlPFC/TP; B dlPFC, precentral gyrus; C Bilateral thalamus; D Supplementary motor area/ dACC. vlPFC ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, TP temporal pole, dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dACC dorsal anterior cingulate cortex

Main effect of Speech act

The effect of “Speech act” formalised versus improvised (Father&Poem > Mother&Santa) generated activity in two right hemisphere clusters: one including dlPFC and inferior frontal cortex and the other composed of inferior parietal cortex (IPC), supramarginal and angular gyrus. In the opposite direction improvised versus formalised speech (Mother&Santa > Father&Poem) showed activity in four left hemispheric clusters including Frontal eye field/Premotor cortex, and vlPFC, SMA/ACC, and dlPFC (see Table 2).

Table 2 Main effect of Speech act

Interactions

The interactions between the two factors “Domain” (religious or secular) and “Speech act” (formalised or improvised) showed activity in the left mPFC in the direction Father&Santa > Mother&Poem while no suprathreshold voxels survived in the opposite direction Mother&Poem > Father&Santa (see Table 3). This interaction shows that the “Domain” factor is affected by the “Speech act” in this region. Particularly, we observed that the activation in these regions is reduced during both religious levels and the formalised secular level but increased during the improvised secular level (see Fig. 2). This region did not overlap with any previous location of main effects analysis.

Table 3 Interactions
Fig. 2
figure 2

Interaction analysis results and contrast estimates

Simple contrasts

Individual contrasts comparing both types of prayers Mother vs. Father, in the direction Mother > Father showed activity in left hemisphere areas including SMA, dACC and vlPFC and TP while the opposite direction, Father > Mother, showed activity in right hemisphere areas including the temporo-parietal junction and dlPFC (see Table 4).

Table 4 Individual contrasts

The comparison between the two improvised speeches showed no activation for Mother > Santa, while the opposite condition Santa > Mother showed increased activity in the left frontal superior lobe. The comparison between the two formalised speeches showed no activation for Father > Poem, while the opposite condition Poem > Father showed an increased activity in the left frontal superior and superior middle lobe and bilateral thalamus. Poem > Father also revealed a very similar cluster to Mother > Father in the left supplementary motor area.

Discussion

The objective of this research was to find the neural activity associated with prayer practices within SYM and also to test whether praying practices within SYM involve social brain regions as has been shown in praying within Christian practices [8]. We observed that, when compared with secular speech, praying involved a reduced activation in the thalamus in both hemispheres of the brain, and reduced activity in the left dlPFC, left vlPFC and left SMA. The same left dlPFC, left vlPFC and left SMA regions were however also decreased in activation for formalised speech versus improvised speech, possibly complying with the necessity of activating these areas for spontaneous speech production. We also observed that the activation in the medial prefrontal cortex was reduced by prayers and formalised secular speech but increased during improvised secular speech. Therefore, these results may potentially reflect the particularities of praying during SYM, where prayers are used to facilitate the later meditation state of mental silence in which practitioners perceive the state of yoga (spiritual union with God), and which may be concomitant with the observed reduced frontal activation which has not appeared in previous fMRI studies of praying in Christians [8, 17].

The results of comparing praying tasks relative to their respective secular tasks (Poem > Father, Santa > Mother or Santa&Poem > Mother&Father) always showed reduced activation in areas of the frontal lobe for both prayer tasks (Mother and Father). It is important to remark that the opposite contrasts displayed no results (Father > Poem, Mother > Santa or Mother&Father > Santa&Poem). We think that these results of deactivation at the frontal lobe during both prayers may constitute a relevant particularity of praying in SYM, especially if we consider that this was not observed with a similar paradigm in Danish Christians [8]. This deactivation of frontal areas while praying in SYM is also interesting considering that most fMRI studies made during religious experiences show greater activity in the frontal lobe [23, 40,41,42]. The deactivation of frontal lobe areas while praying in SYM could be related with the attitude of surrendering to God manifested by practitioners of SYM that, interestingly, seems to be more like the brain activation patterns experienced through intense Islamic Prayers, which was also correlated with decreased activity in frontal areas [43].

Similarly, the interaction analysis revealed a cluster in the mPFC, which was reduced in activation during praying (Father and Mother) and during secular formalized speech (Poem) compared to secular improvised speech (Santa). The mPFC plays a critical role in a wide range of cognitive [44] and emotional [45, 46] social processes; thus, diverse findings in this region have been reported in religious cognition and meditation studies depending on the experimental design and practices analysed [10, 47]. Particularly, the anterior mPFC has consistently appeared in social cognition and theory of mind studies [22], and has been associated with the integration of emotional and moral elevation [46]. The decreased activation in this region by religious and repetitive (Poem) conditions, makes us suggest that this interaction may be associated with the socio emotional regulation´s role of the mPFC. This effect, deactivation on mPFC during prayers, may explain the simple contrast between improvised conditions (Santa > Mother) where we observed a larger superior medial frontal cortex activation, partially overlapping with the mPFC interaction.

A previous study on Christian participants listening to intercessory prayers from speakers who they believed had superior hierarchy within their belief group showed similar deactivations in prefrontal and anterior cingulate regions when compared to less credible speakers [48]. This mPFC region has also been shown to exhibit reduced activation during emotional social processes [49]. Considering prayers are used in SYM to facilitate the state of mental silence, where practitioners experiment a spiritual union with God, we could associate the mPFC finding with the participants' attitude of surrendering towards God, in tune with a decrease of social judgment and ‘mentalizing’ functions reported in this region. Additionally, when the analysis was correlated with the years of the meditation experience, these regions showed larger deactivation. It would thus be interesting to test this effect in a more heterogenous study that included a wider range of experience, e.g., between novice and experienced practitioners of SYM.

In the main effect of secular speech, the deactivation of the thalamus during both prayers compared to secular conditions, could be explained by the role the thalamus in the conscious perception of the surroundings, controlling the flow of sensory information to the cortex [50]. Therefore, this deactivation could be linked to the inner concentration established during the prayers in order to reach a meditative state, reinforcing the downgrading of external distractors. Our previous functional connectivity study in SYM found a reduction in functional connectivity between the thalamus and the mPFC during the meditation state that was related to the depth of mental silence perceived by the subjects [38]. The thalamus has furthermore been shown to be reduced in activity in previous studies of different meditation techniques (yoga, transcendental meditation and mindfulness meditation), which associated the thalamic deactivation with reduction of pain perception, associating it with a filtering effect inflecting the sensory information [51,52,53]. This reduced thalamic activity was also found when comparing formalised secular speech with formalised prayer (Poem > Father), and perhaps could suggest a deeper focus during formal praying, but the effect did not appear in the equivalent improvised comparison (Santa > Mother) or in the contrast between the two kinds of prayers (Mother vs. Father).

In our study, the thalamus deactivation appeared to be the only cluster that separated the religious and secular speech conditions, since activations in the vlPFC, dlPFC and SMA were also observed in the contrast of improvised versus formalised speech (Santa & Mother > Poem & Father); some of these activation clusters also appeared in the simple contrast between improvised prayer versus formalised prayer (see Fig. 3), suggesting that improvised speech involved the activation of the left inferior frontal cortex (i.e., the Broca area), as well as the left SMA/dACC [54] and left dlPFC [55,56,57] in line with the known involvement of left hemisphere areas in language processes [58]. Thus, we observed that improvised speech involved left perisylvian brain regions, like the vlPFC which may be related to the social brain as well as language processes. Some of these regions also appeared in the simple contrast between formalised conditions, where the secular condition presented larger activation of the SMA and left dlPFC, which could be interpreted as a lower formal (automatic) effect of the poem in meditators, who pray frequently while the poem was, in some cases, particularly trained for the experiment.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Main effects of improvised speech (Santa&Mother > Poem&Father) (blue), Mother > Father (red) and their overlaps (purple). A SMA; B vlPFC; C dlPFC; D Frontal eye Field/Pre-motor cortex. SMA supplementary motor area, vlPFC ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

In line with Schjoedt previous findings in Christian prayers [8], we also found no activation during prayers compared to secular recitations, which was also found in the individual contrasts comparing each prayer with its corresponding secular equivalent. The opposite contrast (Santa & Poem > Mother & Father) recruited the left dlPFC in both studies but also revealed several differences; while we obtained left hemispheric activation in the vlPFC, SMA, and thalamus, the original study from Schjoedt et al. showed right frontal activations alongside bilateral parieto-occipital activity, associating the bilateral frontal activity to an effect on the executive and attentional networks related to the participants’ different levels of familiarity with the two domains [8].

We also observed activations in the right dlPFC and right IPC associated with the formalised speech acts (Poem & Father) in SYM (see Fig. 4). This effect was also observed when comparing formalised prayer with both improvised conditions, but it cannot be uniquely associated with formalised prayer since it was not shown when comparing between the formalised speech acts. Similar regions were previously observed and associated with a general effect of rehearsal and retrieval [8], which further shows the aforementioned difference of familiarity between the formalised tasks. Therefore, we may suggest that formalised speech involves right lateralized brain regions such as the IPC and the dlPFC while improvised speech involved left hemispheric activations in vlPFC, dlPFC and SMA (see Figs. 3 and 4). Future studies exploring lateralization effects of formal prayer in social and language related brain regions may be of interest under the support of a strong hypothesis that is absent at this moment as far as we know.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Main effects of formalised speech (Poem&Father > Santa&Mother) (blue), Father > Mother (red) and their overlaps (purple). A dlPFC; B IPC. dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, IPC inferior parietal cortex

Limitations

The current study has several limitations. One limitation is the small sample size of volunteers participating in this study due to the low number of expert practitioners of SYM available in Tenerife. The second limitation concerns the absence of a resting task condition, as we used counting backwards as a baseline condition, mainly because an important aim was to replicate the study of Schjoedt [8]. However, this contrast with a mentally very demanding task could have diminished activation findings for the prayer and speech conditions. Additionally, some participants particularly trained the poem for this experiment, which may have negatively affected the results since participants have more practice with the formalised prayer. Finally, the quality of cognitive performance during the fMRI task could not be tested inside the scanner given the silent language conditions involved, which did not include any other behavioural record. We tried to control for task performance during fMRI acquisition using a self-report questionnaire after the scan session to reduce this limitation.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research of the devotional-prayer aspect of SYM showed reduced frontal activity, particularly a deactivation in the mPFC during prayer, which may be related to the latter state of mental silence where participants perceive the state of yoga, which as previously mentioned is considered the spiritual union with God, showing their trust in the receiver of their speech (i.e., God or the founder of SYM).

We found several brain areas associated with social cognition to be involved in praying in SYM, similar to the ones presented in our study of reference, the study of Christian prayer [8]; however, the pattern of activation was different which could suggest that SYM prayer cognition differs from Christian prayer cognition. Different forms of praying appear to have some common elements but also reveal distinct cognitive elements and hence may have different regions of activation depending on the religious content and speech act. It would therefore be interesting to directly compare prayers in two or more different religious traditions.

Finally, the deactivation of the bilateral thalamus and frontal areas during both prayers compared to secular conditions, could be the main characterization of prayers in SYM, which could be explained by the role of the thalamus for the conscious perception of the surroundings, which is not necessary during praying and hence deactivated, and the surrendering attitude towards God, reflected in the deactivation of prefrontal regions. Therefore, these deactivations could be linked to the inner concentration and devotion during prayers that is part of the meditative process in SYM.

Materials and methods

Participants

Sixteen expert practitioners of Sahaja Yoga meditation volunteered to participate in this study (10 females; 1 left-handed; age range 24–63 years). All participants had more than 7 years of daily practice experience in this meditation. Volunteers reported (in a 5-point Likert scale) their experience in SYM, including among other things their frequency of praying the Lord’s Prayer and prayers to the Divine Mother, the average time dedicated to meditation per day and the frequency of the perception of the state of mental silence. After the scan session, participants were asked to assess the quality of their performance inside the scanner, and they had to complete a questionnaire on religious beliefs and practices, including belief in God, confidence in God’s reciprocity and frequency of praying (see Table 5). None of the participants reported any physical or mental illness, nor any history of neurological disorders, addiction to alcohol, nicotine or drugs.

Table 5 Demographic data and meditative and religious characteristics of the group

Conditions and procedure

We designed a stimulus paradigm with five conditions based on Schjoedt’s previous neuroimaging study on prayer in Christian practitioners who belonged to a fraction within the Danish Lutheran Church called the Inner Mission [8, 17]. Four of the conditions followed a two-by-two factorial design between ‘Domain’ (spiritual vs. secular) and ‘Speech-act’ (formalised vs. improvised) factors (levels) (See Table 6). For the formalised spiritual speech-act, we used the recitation of the Lord’s Prayer (Father), and for the improvised spiritual speech-act, we used a prayer to the Divine Mother (Mother). For the secular speech acts, we used the recitation of a formalised well-known poem (Poem) and making improvised wishes to Santa Claus (Santa). We also included a control condition, similar to Schjoedt’s paradigm [8, 17], in which participants were asked to count backwards from 100 rather than a resting state baseline condition to avoid task versus resting related effects.

Table 6 Two-by-two factorial design

In SYM, the Lord’s Prayer is mainly used by meditators when they focus their attention at the 6th center (called in ancient yoga Agnya Chakra) theoretically located according to SYM at the optic chiasm although meditators focus their attention on their forehead. This 6th center is related to the quality of forgiveness and the state of thoughtless awareness or mental silence [59]. On the other hand, the prayer to the Divine Mother is related to the 4th center (called in ancient yoga Anahata Chakra) located at the cardiac plexus and meditators put their attention around their heart zone; according to SYM the qualities of this 4th center among others are love, compassion, feeling of security and fearlessness [59] (some examples of prayer to the Divine Mother, written by participants of this study, and a diagram with the location of the chakras and their qualities according to SYM are available as supplementary data. See Additional file 1).

A prior 7 min of structural scanning was used to instruct volunteers to enter into meditation in order to facilitate the adaptation to the MRI scanner and also to simulate a normal SYM prayer condition which are normally recited inside a meditation state. Afterwards volunteers were asked to perform the already mentioned five tasks in a pseudo-randomized order. Each task was preceded by their respective two seconds auditory instruction. Each of the five conditions was repeated six times, each of them lasted 26 s, excluding the 2 s audio instruction. The total time of fMRI acquisition was 14 min. During this time, tasks were performed silently as internal speech with eyes closed, and participants were asked to concentrate on the task. They were instructed to repeat the current task if they finished it before the end of the 26 s block.

MRI acquisition

Axially oriented blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) functional images were obtained by a 3T Sigma HD MR scanner (GE Healthcare, Waukesha, WI, USA) using an echo-planar-imaging gradient-echo sequence and an 8-channel head coil with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 21,6 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 64 × 64 pixels, 37 slices, 4 × 4 mm in plane resolution, slice thickness = 4 mm, interleaved acquisition. The head was stabilized with foam pads. The slices were aligned to the anterior commissure—posterior commissure line and covered the whole brain. Functional scanning was preceded by 20 s of dummy scans to ensure tissue steady-state magnetization. A total of 420 fMRI whole brain volumes were taken during each participant’s single run.

Prior to the fMRI acquisition, high-resolution sagittal oriented anatomical images were collected for anatomical reference, for this purpose a 3D fast spoiled-gradient-recalled pulse sequence was obtained with the following parameters: TR = 8.844 ms, TE = 1.752 ms, flip angle = 10°, matrix size = 256 × 256 pixels, slice thickness = 1 mm.

Data preprocessing and analysis

The functional brain images were pre-processed and analysed using Statistical Parametric Mapping, SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London United Kingdom). A general linear model was implemented for each participant at the first level effects, and the results were computed as a group (second-level effects).

Pre-processing steps included: (1) within-subject registration and unwarping of time series, the realignment part of this routine realigns a time-series of images acquired from the same subject using a least squares approach and a 6 parameter (rigid body) spatial transformation; (2) co-registering individual structural images to the mean functional image of each subject; (3) co-registered images were segmented into grey matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, bone, soft tissue and air/background; (4) spatial normalization of functional volumes by using the parameters extracted from the anatomical segmentation procedure in each subject and resampling voxel size to 2 × 2 × 2 mm; and (5) spatial smoothing with an 8-mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel, a high pass filter was used to remove low-frequency drifts in the fMRI BOLD signal. Only one subject presented extensive head movement (more than 3 mm/3º), but movement was properly corrected and, thus, we included the subject in the analyses. We also tested whether the exclusion of this participant affected the group statistics and that was not the case.

The first level analysis included 6 conditions: baseline condition (counting backwards), the audio instruction of each task (which was not used for posterior analysis), and the four conditions of interest: Santa, Poem, Father and Mother. The contrasts of interest computed were as follows: (1) Domain main effect (Mother&Father > Santa&Poem and vice versa); (2) Speech act main effect (Poem&Father > Santa&Mother and vice versa). Next, we tested for interactions (Mother&Poem > Father&Santa and vice versa), simple effects (comparing Mother, Father, Santa and Poem with each other), and simple contrasts comparing each condition with the base-line condition (Count). Additionally, this analysis included the head motion parameters as regressors of no interest in the model. Group analysis was performed by using the random-effect approach with a one-sample t test.

We also explored the brain–behaviour relationships by a regression analysis with the frequency of practice (frequency of the Lord’s Prayer recitation, frequency of the Mother’s prayer and the frequency of mental silence perception) and the meditation experience as independent variables and brain activation as dependent variable. These analyses did not present extensive changes compared to the presented results and any relevant effect is discussed. Analyses were thresholded at a statistical voxel-wise p < 0.001 and at corrected cluster p-value with multiple correction p < 0.05 FWE.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BOLD:

Blood-oxygen-level-dependent

dlPFC:

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

dmPFC:

Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex

dACC:

Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

mPFC:

Medial prefrontal cortex

SMA:

Supplementary motor area

SYM:

Sahaja Yoga Meditation

TE:

Echo time

TP:

Temporal pole

TPJ:

Temporoparietal junction

TR:

Repetition time

vlPFC:

Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex

References

  1. Rubia K. The neurobiology of meditation and its clinical effectiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychol. 2009;82(1):1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Fox K, Nijeboer S, Dixon ML, Floman JL, Ellamil M, Rumak SP, et al. Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;43:48–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Fox K, Dixon ML, Nijeboer S, Girn M, Floman JL, Lifshitz M, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: a review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;1(65):208–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Kokodoko A. The Yoga Sutra of Patanjali. Libr. J. 2014. 139–196.

  5. Hendriks T. The effects of Sahaja Yoga meditation on mental health: a systematic review. J Complement Integr Med. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1515/jcim-2016-0163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Koenig HG. Medicine, religion, and health: where science and spirituality meet. West Conshohocken: Templeton Foundation; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bulbulia J. Schjoedt U. The neural basis of religion. 2016;28:35–55.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Schjoedt U, Stødkilde-Jørgensen H, Geertz AW, Roepstorff A. Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4(2):199–207.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Seitz RJ, Paloutzian RF, Angel HF. From believing to belief: a general theoretical model. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018;30(9):1254–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Grafman J, Cristofori I, Zhong W, Bulbulia J. The neural basis of religious cognition. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2020;29(2):126–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Rim JI, Ojeda JC, Svob C, Kayser J, Drews E, Kim Y, et al. Current understanding of religion, spirituality, and their neurobiological correlates. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2019;27(5):303–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Cristofori I, Bulbulia J, Shaver JH, Wilson M, Krueger F, Grafman J. Neural correlates of mystical experience. Neuropsychologia. 2016;8(80):212–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Urgesi C, Aglioti SM, Skrap M, Fabbro F. The spiritual brain: selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence. Neuron. 2010;65(3):309–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Christensen JF, Flexas A, de Miguel P, Cela-Conde CJ, Munar E. Roman Catholic beliefs produce characteristic neural responses to moral dilemmas. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(2):240–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Han S, Gu X, Mao L, Ge J, Wang G, Ma Y. Neural substrates of self-referential processing in Chinese Buddhists. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2010;5(2–3):332–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Han S, Mao L, Gu X, Zhu Y, Ge J, Ma Y. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Soc Neurosci. 2008;3(1):1–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Schjoedt U, Stødkilde-Jørgensen H, Geertz AW, Roepstorff A. Rewarding prayers. Neurosci Lett. 2008;443(3):165–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kober SE, Witte M, Ninaus M, Koschutnig K, Wiesen D, Zaiser G, et al. Ability to gain control over one’s own brain activity and its relation to spiritual practice: a multimodal imaging study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;24(11):271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind.’ Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7(2):77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage. 2004;22(4):1694–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Vogeley K, May M, Ritzl A, Falkai P, Zilles K, Fink GR. Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(5):817–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(4):268–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Azari NP, Nickel J, Wunderlich G, Niedeggen M, Hefter H, Tellmann L, et al. Neural correlates of religious experience. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13(8):1649–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Azari NP, Missimer J, Seitz RJ. Religious experience and emotion: evidence for distinctive cognitive neural patterns. Int J Phytoremediation. 2005;21(1):263–81.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Völlm BA, Taylor ANW, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, et al. Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage. 2006;29(1):90–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Nirmala Devi SM. The book of Adi Shakti. La Cultura della Madre; 2013.

  27. Nirmala Devi SM. Meta Modern Era. Lulu.com; 2018.

  28. Hernández SE, Suero J, Rubia K, González-Mora JL. Monitoring the neural activity of the state of mental silence while practicing sahaja yoga meditation. J Alter Compl Med. 2015;21(3):175–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Tomasino B, Fregona S, Skrap M, Fabbro F. Meditation-related activations are modulated by the practices needed to obtain it and by the expertise: an ALE meta-analysis study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00346.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. McCauley RN, Lawson ET. Bringing Ritual to Mind. Bringing Ritual to Mind: Cambridge University Press; 2002.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  31. Whitehouse H. Arguments and Icons: Divergent Modes of Religiosity. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  32. Whitehouse H. Modes of religiosity. Bulletin Council Soc Study Reli. 2008;1(37):108.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Berkovich-Ohana A, Wilf M, Kahana R, Arieli A, Malach R. Repetitive speech elicits widespread deactivation in the human cortex: the “Mantra” effect? Brain Behav. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.346.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Bayles J, Vanderloo JP. I’ve forgotten how to pray. J Miss State Med Assoc. 2016;57(2):43–4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Silveira S, Bao Y, Wang L, Pöppel E, Avram M, Simmank F, et al. Does a bishop pray when he prays? And does his brain distinguish between different religions? Psych J. 2015;4(4):199–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Buckner RL, Vincent JL. Unrest at rest: default activity and spontaneous network correlations. Neuroimage. 2007;37(4):1091–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Hernández SE, Suero J, Barros A, González-Mora JL, Rubia K. Increased grey matter associated with long-Term Sahaja yoga meditation: A voxel-based morphometry study. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(3):1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Hernández SE, Barros-Loscertales A, Xiao Y, González-Mora JL, Rubia K. Gray matter and functional connectivity in anterior cingulate cortex are associated with the state of mental silence during sahaja yoga meditation. Neuroscience. 2018;371:395–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Hernández SE, Dorta R, Suero J, Barros-Loscertales A, González-Mora JL, Rubia K. Larger whole brain grey matter associated with long-term Sahaja Yoga Meditation: a detailed area by area comparison. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(12):1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Newberg A, Pourdehnad M, Alavi A, D’Aquili EG. Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: preliminary findings and methodological issues. Percept Mot Skills. 2003;97(2):625–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. De Araujo DB, Ribeiro S, Cecchi GA, Carvalho FM, Sanchez TA, Pinto JP, et al. Seeing with the eyes shut: neural basis of enhanced imagery following ayahuasca ingestion. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(11):2550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Beauregard M, Paquette V. Neural correlates of mystical experience in carmelite nuns. Neurosci Lett. 2006;1(405):186–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Newberg AB, Wintering NA, Yaden DB, Waldman MR, Reddin J, Alavi A. A case series study of the neurophysiological effects of altered states of mind during intense Islamic prayer. J Physiol Paris. 2015;109(4–6):214–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Adenzato M, Brambilla M, Manenti R, De Lucia L, Trojano L, Garofalo S, et al. Gender differences in cognitive theory of Mind revealed by transcranial direct current stimulation on medial prefrontal cortex. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Cheng Q, Cui X, Lin J, Weng X, Mo L. Neural correlates of moral goodness and moral beauty judgments. Brain Res. 2020;1(1726): 146534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Avram M, Hennig-Fast K, Bao Y, Pöppel E, Reiser M, Blautzik J, et al. Neural correlates of moral judgments in first- and third-person perspectives: Implications for neuroethics and beyond. BMC Neurosci. 2014;15(1):1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-15-39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Cooper AC, Ventura B, Northoff G. Beyond the veil of duality—topographic reorganization model of meditation. Neurosci Conscious. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niac013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Schjoedt U, Stødkilde-Jørgensen H, Geertz AW, Lund TE, Roepstorff A. The power of charisma—perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive network of believers in intercessory prayer. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6(1):119–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Bartels A, Zeki S. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage. 2004;21(3):1155–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Basso MA, Uhlrich D, Bickford ME. Cortical function: a view from the Thalamus. Neuron. 2005;45(4):485–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kakigi R, Nakata H, Inui K, Hiroe N, Nagata O, Honda M, et al. Intracerebral pain processing in a Yoga Master who claims not to feel pain during meditation. Eur J Pain. 2005;9(5):581–581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.12.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Orme-Johnson DW, Schneider RH, Son YD, Nidich S, Cho ZH. Neuroimaging of meditation’s effect on brain reactivity to pain. NeuroReport. 2006;17(12):1359–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, Gordon NS, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC. Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation. J Neurosci. 2011;31(14):5540–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Alderson-Day B, Fernyhough C. Inner speech: Development, cognitive functions, phenomenology, and neurobiology. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(5):931–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Hodgson VJ, Lambon Ralph MA, Jackson RL. Multiple dimensions underlying the functional organization of the language network. Neuroimage. 2021;1(241): 118444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak JRS. Investigating a network model of word generation with positron emission tomography. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1991;244(1310):101–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Shergill SS, Brammer MJ, Fukuda R, Bullmore E, Amaro E, Murray RM, et al. Modulation of activity in temporal cortex during generation of inner speech. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;16(4):219–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houdé O, et al. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage. 2006;30(4):1414–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hendriks T, Pritikin J, Choudhary R, Danyluck C. Exploring the relationship between character strengths and meditation: a cross-sectional study among long-term practitioners of sahaja yoga meditation. Int J Appl Posit Psychol. 2022;7(1):31–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the support of MRI services for Biomedical Studies (Servicio de Resonancia Magnética para Investigaciones Biomédicas) of the University of La Laguna. We warmly thank all the volunteers for their participation in this study.

Funding

ABL has received research support from the Universitat Jaume I (UJI-B2020-23). JLGM was supported by fundings: Grant ProID2021010096 from Agencia Canaria de Investigación, Innovación y sociedad de la Información. Grant MAC2/1.1b/352 (MACBIOIDI-II) from GAC and INTERREG MAC. KR was supported by NIHR grants (project ref: NIHR130077; NIHR203684), and by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) and the UK Department of Health via the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) for Mental Health at South London and the Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the funders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

OPD analysed and interpreted the data as well as contributing with most of the writing and review of the manuscript. ABL assisted with the interpretation of the results and the review of the manuscript. US, JGM, KR, JS have substantively revised the draft. SEH assisted with the interpretation of the results and the review of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Alfonso Barrós-Loscertales or Sergio Elías Hernández.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Prior to participation, participants were given information about the study, and all participants signed a written informed consent form according to the principles of the Helsinki Declaration. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations as approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of La Laguna.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

KR has received a grant from TAKEDA for another project and consulting fees from SUPERNUS and LUNDBECK. The other authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Examples of spontaneous mother’s prayer, written by participants of this study.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Perez-Diaz, O., Barrós-Loscertales, A., Schjoedt, U. et al. Monitoring the neural activity associated with praying in Sahaja Yoga meditation. BMC Neurosci 24, 61 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-023-00828-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-023-00828-x

Keywords